Learning To Make Music Enhances Spatial Reasoning.

Does active instruction in music enhance preschool and elementary students' performance on spatial tasks? Researchers have been interested in this question not only because of the importance of spatial abilities, which underlie professions such as engineering, archaeology, and surgery, but also because spatial abilities are thought to be important in the discipline of mathematics.' This meta-analytic review synthesizes the results of 15 independent studies that address this controversial and highly publicized question. While the studies summarized here address a question distinct from the "Mozart effect" studies,2 scholars and the public have often failed to distinguish these two bodies of research (one on the spatial effects of passive music listening, the other on the spatial effects of active participation in music instruction). Yet whether college students' performance on spatial tasks is temporarily improved following brief exposure to classical music tells us almost nothing about whether preschool and elementary children perform better on spatial tests following a program of active music instruction. These instruction studies deserve separate summary so that we can understand their meaning and determine their implications for future research and classroom practice. Several previous reviews have assessed music's impact on nonmusical abilities, but none has directly addressed the question posed by this review. Five of these reviews are loosely related to the present summary in focus and/or method. A narrative review by Wolff assessed nonmusical outcomes of music education and critiqued study methods, including several studies with spatial outcomes.3 Tunks's chapter in The Handbook of Research on Music Teaching and Learning defined and summarized the concept of transfer in relation to the field of music education.4 The only meta-analysis on extramusical effects of music education was published in 1996 by Standley, but it does not include spatial outcomes. Instead, it examines the effects of music used as a reinforcement for various educational and thera-

[1]  R. Glassman Keeping Mozart in Mind.ByGordon L Shaw.San Diego (California): Academic Press. $49.95. xx + 374 p + 8 pl; ill.; index. ISBN: 0–12–639290–0. [CD‐ROM included.] 2000. , 2002 .

[2]  Terry D. Bilhartz,et al.  The effect of early music training on child cognitive development , 1999 .

[3]  Eugenia Costa-Giomi The Effects of Three Years of Piano Instruction on Children's Cognitive Development , 1999 .

[4]  Gordon L. Shaw,et al.  Keeping Mozart in Mind , 1999 .

[5]  Christopher F. Chabris,et al.  Prelude or requiem for the ‘Mozart effect’? , 1999, Nature.

[6]  Bennett Reimer Facing the Risks of the “Mozart Effect” , 1999 .

[7]  G L Shaw,et al.  Enhanced learning of proportional math through music training and spatial-temporal training. , 1999, Neurological research.

[8]  Daniel L. Schwartz,et al.  Chapter 3: Rethinking Transfer: A Simple Proposal With Multiple Implications , 1999 .

[9]  Joyce Eastlund Gromko,et al.  The Effect of Music Training on Preschoolers' Spatial-Temporal Task Performance , 1998 .

[10]  F. Rauscher,et al.  Key Components of the Mozart Effect , 1998, Perceptual and motor skills.

[11]  Frederick Mosteller,et al.  The Tennessee Study of Class Size in the Early School Grades , 1997 .

[12]  Frederick Mosteller,et al.  Sustained Inquiry in Education: Lessons from Skill Grouping and Class Size , 1996 .

[13]  J. Standley,et al.  A Meta-Analysis on the Effects of Music as Reinforcement for Education/Therapy Objectives , 1996 .

[14]  John W. Flohr Children's Electrophysiological Responses to Music. , 1996 .

[15]  J. Newman,et al.  An Experimental Test of “The Mozart Effect”: Does Listening to His Music Improve Spatial Ability? , 1995, Perceptual and motor skills.

[16]  Robert Rosenthal,et al.  WRITING META-ANALYTIC REVIEWS , 1995 .

[17]  J. Staiger,et al.  Increased corpus callosum size in musicians , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  Stephen W. Smoliar,et al.  The Mind Behind the Musical Ear: How Children Develop Musical , 1995 .

[19]  Frances H. Rauscher Music and Spatial Task Performance: A Causal Relationship. , 1994 .

[20]  Marianne Hassler,et al.  Creative musical behavior and sex hormones: Musical talent and spatial ability in the two sexes , 1992, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[21]  M Hassler,et al.  Maturation rate and spatial, verbal, and musical abilities: a seven-year-longitudinal study. , 1991, The International journal of neuroscience.

[22]  M. M. Kelly,et al.  Effects of Visual-Spatial Added Cues on Fourth-Graders' Melodic Discrimination , 1989 .

[23]  D. Perkins,et al.  Rocky Roads to Transfer: Rethinking Mechanism of a Neglected Phenomenon , 1989 .

[24]  R. Rosenthal Pygmalion Effects: Existence, Magnitude, and Social Importance , 1987 .

[25]  N. Birbaumer,et al.  Musical Talent and Visual-Spatial Ability: Onset of Puberty , 1987 .

[26]  Niels Birbaumer,et al.  Witelson's dichaptic stimulation test and children with different levels of musical talent , 1986, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  M. Linn,et al.  Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: a meta-analysis. , 1985, Child development.

[28]  C. Hobbs A Comparison of the Music Aptitude, Scholastic Aptitude, and Academic Achievement of Young Children , 1985 .

[29]  M. Hassler,et al.  Musical Talent and Visual-Spatial Abilities: A Longitudinal Study , 1985 .

[30]  J. Fodor,et al.  The Modularity of Mind: An Essay on Faculty Psychology , 1984 .

[31]  J. Gustafsson A unifying model for the structure of intellectual abilities , 1984 .

[32]  Donald B. Rubin,et al.  Interpersonal expectancy effects: the first 345 studies , 1978, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[33]  John J. O'Malley,et al.  Training in Musical Rhythm and Field Dependence of Children , 1975, Perceptual and motor skills.

[34]  Peter H. Wolff,et al.  Nonmusicol Effects of the Kodaly Music Curriculum in Primary Grade Children , 1975 .

[35]  H. Barker,et al.  Cognitive Pattern Perception and Musical Performance , 1973, Perceptual and motor skills.

[36]  Klára Kokas Psychological Testing in Hungarian Music Education , 1969 .

[37]  L. Taetle The effect of active and passive music instruction on the spatial ability of kindergarten children , 1999 .

[38]  Daniel L. Schwartz,et al.  Rethinking transfer: A simple proposal with multiple implica-tions , 1999 .

[39]  D. Rubin,et al.  Contrasts and Effect Sizes in Behavioral Research , 1999 .

[40]  J. Bruer The Myth of the First Three Years , 1999 .

[41]  Paul M. Wortman,et al.  Judging research quality. , 1994 .

[42]  Thomas W. Tunks The transfer of music learning , 1992 .

[43]  Rivvy. Meloff The Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale, fourth edition: a descriptive study , 1987 .

[44]  R. Rosenthal,et al.  Contrast Analysis: Focused Comparisons in the Analysis of Variance , 1985 .

[45]  W. Hays Summing Up: The Science of Reviewing Research. , 1985 .

[46]  Magda Kalmár The effects of music education based on Kodaly's directives in nursery school children: From a psychologist's point of view. , 1982 .

[47]  T. Cook,et al.  Quasi-experimentation: Design & analysis issues for field settings , 1979 .

[48]  David Wechsler,et al.  Manual for the Wechsler intelligence scale for children , 1974 .

[49]  Dorothea Agnes McCarthy,et al.  Manual for the McCarthy scales of children's abilities , 1972 .

[50]  D. Wechsler Manual for the Wechsler preschool and primary scale of intelligence , 1967 .