Beyond the language given: the neural correlates of inferring speaker meaning.

Even though language allows us to say exactly what we mean, we often use language to say things indirectly, in a way that depends on the specific communicative context. For example, we can use an apparently straightforward sentence like "It is hard to give a good presentation" to convey deeper meanings, like "Your talk was a mess!" One of the big puzzles in language science is how listeners work out what speakers really mean, which is a skill absolutely central to communication. However, most neuroimaging studies of language comprehension have focused on the arguably much simpler, context-independent process of understanding direct utterances. To examine the neural systems involved in getting at contextually constrained indirect meaning, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging as people listened to indirect replies in spoken dialog. Relative to direct control utterances, indirect replies engaged dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, right temporo-parietal junction and insula, as well as bilateral inferior frontal gyrus and right medial temporal gyrus. This suggests that listeners take the speaker's perspective on both cognitive (theory of mind) and affective (empathy-like) levels. In line with classic pragmatic theories, our results also indicate that currently popular "simulationist" accounts of language comprehension fail to explain how listeners understand the speaker's intended message.

[1]  T. Hendler,et al.  An fMRI investigation of the neural correlates underlying the processing of novel metaphoric expressions , 2007, Brain and Language.

[2]  R Saxe,et al.  People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.

[3]  R. Mar The neural bases of social cognition and story comprehension. , 2011, Annual review of psychology.

[4]  Marcel Adam Just,et al.  The Role of the Theory-of-Mind Cortical Network in the Comprehension of Narratives , 2009, Lang. Linguistics Compass.

[5]  Evelyn C. Ferstl,et al.  The extended language network: A meta‐analysis of neuroimaging studies on text comprehension , 2008, Human brain mapping.

[6]  Rebecca Saxe,et al.  Overlapping and non-overlapping brain regions for theory of mind and self reflection in individual subjects. , 2006, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[7]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[8]  J C Mazziotta,et al.  Reafferent copies of imitated actions in the right superior temporal cortex , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[9]  Lindsey J. Powell,et al.  It's the Thought That Counts , 2006, Psychological science.

[10]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  Playing Charades in the fMRI: Are Mirror and/or Mentalizing Areas Involved in Gestural Communication? , 2009, PloS one.

[11]  Laurence R. Horn,et al.  The handbook of pragmatics , 2004 .

[12]  H. Grice Logic and conversation , 1975 .

[13]  E. Goffman Interaction Ritual: Essays on Face-To-Face Behavior , 1967 .

[14]  Niall W. Duncan,et al.  Is there a core neural network in empathy? An fMRI based quantitative meta-analysis , 2011, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[15]  Paul Boersma,et al.  Praat, a system for doing phonetics by computer , 2002 .

[16]  C. Frith,et al.  Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[17]  Edward T. Bullmore,et al.  Specialization of right temporo-parietal junction for mentalizing and its relation to social impairments in autism , 2011, NeuroImage.

[18]  A. Bailey,et al.  Are there theory of mind regions in the brain? A review of the neuroimaging literature , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[19]  Alvin I. Goldman,et al.  Précis of Simulating Minds: The Philosophy, Psychology, and Neuroscience of Mindreading , 2006 .

[20]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Dissociable Medial Prefrontal Contributions to Judgments of Similar and Dissimilar Others , 2006, Neuron.

[21]  Penelope Brown,et al.  Politeness: Some Universals in Language Usage , 1989 .

[22]  Thomas Holtgraves,et al.  Comprehending indirect replies : When and how are their conveyed meanings activated? , 1999 .

[23]  M. Just,et al.  Differentiable cortical networks for inferences concerning people's intentions versus physical causality , 2011, Human brain mapping.

[24]  D. Sperber,et al.  Relevance: Communication and cognition, 2nd ed. , 1995 .

[25]  T. Dalgleish The emotional brain , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[26]  P. Strick,et al.  Imaging the premotor areas , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[27]  Nelleke Oostdijk,et al.  Het Corpus Gesproken Nederlands , 1999 .

[28]  R. Saxe Against simulation: the argument from error , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[29]  Jiang Xu,et al.  Language in context: emergent features of word, sentence, and narrative comprehension , 2005, NeuroImage.

[30]  Uta Frith,et al.  Theory of mind , 2001, Current Biology.

[31]  Phillip J. Holcomb,et al.  Making sense of discourse: An fMRI study of causal inferencing across sentences , 2006, NeuroImage.

[32]  J. Decety,et al.  Effect of subjective perspective taking during simulation of action: a PET investigation of agency , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[33]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. , 1999, Science.

[34]  Keith Oatley,et al.  The Function of Fiction is the Abstraction and Simulation of Social Experience , 2008, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[35]  C. Price The anatomy of language: contributions from functional neuroimaging , 2000, Journal of anatomy.

[36]  J. Decety To What Extent is the Experience of Empathy Mediated by Shared Neural Circuits? , 2010 .

[37]  Rutvik H. Desai,et al.  The neurobiology of semantic memory , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[38]  L. Fadiga,et al.  Active perception: sensorimotor circuits as a cortical basis for language , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[39]  T. Singer,et al.  The Social Neuroscience of Empathy , 2009, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[40]  E. Bizzi,et al.  The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1996 .

[41]  R. Saxe Uniquely human social cognition , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[42]  C. Frith,et al.  Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[43]  Peter Hagoort,et al.  When Elephants Fly: Differential Sensitivity of Right and Left Inferior Frontal Gyri to Discourse and World Knowledge , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[44]  M. Pickering,et al.  An integrated theory of language production and comprehension. , 2013, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[45]  D. Sperber,et al.  Relevance: Communication and Cognition , 1997 .

[46]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror neuron system. , 2009, Archives of neurology.

[47]  Daniel Tranel,et al.  The Insula and Evaluative Processes , 2011, Psychological science.

[48]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[49]  R. Saxe The neural evidence for simulation is weaker than I think you think it is , 2009 .