Infant attention to intentional action predicts preschool theory of mind.

This research examines whether there are continuities between infant social attention and later theory of mind. Forty-five children were studied as infants and then again as 4-year-olds. Measures of infant social attention (decrement of attention during habituation to displays of intentional action) significantly predicted later theory of mind (false-belief understanding). Possibly, this longitudinal association could have been explained by more general developments in IQ, verbal competence, or executive function (rather than continuities in the realm of social cognition). However, the association remained significant and undiminished even when IQ, verbal competence, and executive function were controlled. The findings thus provide strong support for an important continuity in social cognition separable from continuities in more general information processing.

[1]  R. Mccall What Process Mediates Predictions of Childhood IQ from Infant Habituation and Recognition Memory? Speculations on the Roles of Inhibition and Rate of Information Processing. , 1994 .

[2]  F. Happé Understanding Minds and Metaphors: Insights from the Study of Figurative Language in Autism , 1995 .

[3]  J. Dunn,et al.  Theory of mind, emotion understanding, language, and family background: individual differences and interrelations. , 1999, Child development.

[4]  J. Feldman,et al.  Individual differences in infants' information processing: reliability, stability, and prediction. , 1988, Child development.

[5]  Z. Nadasdy,et al.  Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age , 1995, Cognition.

[6]  J. Astington,et al.  A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory-of-mind development. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[7]  H. Wellman,et al.  Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. , 2001, Child development.

[8]  M. A. Merrill,et al.  Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale , 1972 .

[9]  H. Wellman,et al.  Knowledge acquisition in foundational domains. , 1998 .

[10]  M. Bornstein Habituation of attention as a measure of visual information processing in human infants: Summary , 1985 .

[11]  C. Moore,et al.  Children's understanding of the modal expression of speaker certainty and uncertainty and its relation to the development of a representational theory of mind. , 1990, Child development.

[12]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Understanding attention: 12- and 18-month-olds know what is new for other persons. , 2003, Developmental psychology.

[13]  H. Wellman,et al.  Infants' ability to connect gaze and emotional expression to intentional action , 2002, Cognition.

[14]  I. Sigel,et al.  HANDBOOK OF CHILD PSYCHOLOGY , 2006 .

[15]  Joseph F. Fagan,et al.  Infant Recognition Memory and Later Intelligence. , 1981 .

[16]  G. Kochanska,et al.  Inhibitory control in young children and its role in emerging internalization. , 1996, Child development.

[17]  A. Woodward Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach , 1998, Cognition.

[18]  G. Butterworth,et al.  The development of sensory, motor and cognitive capacities in early infancy : from perception to cognition , 1998 .

[19]  A. Meltzoff Understanding the Intentions of Others: Re-Enactment of Intended Acts by 18-Month-Old Children. , 1995, Developmental psychology.

[20]  L. Moses,et al.  Individual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind. , 2001, Child development.

[21]  F. Happé The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. , 1995, Child development.

[22]  R. Mccall,et al.  A meta-analysis of infant habituation and recognition memory performance as predictors of later IQ. , 1993, Child development.

[23]  J. Astington,et al.  Cognitive Factors and Family Structure Associated with Theory of Mind Development in Young Children. , 1996 .

[24]  Jodie A. Baird,et al.  Infants parse dynamic action. , 2001, Child development.

[25]  Terri Gullickson,et al.  Annals of Child Development , 1995 .

[26]  Norman A. Krasnegor,et al.  Measurement of Audition and Vision in the First Year of Postnatal Life: A Methodological Overview , 1985 .

[27]  A. Gopnik,et al.  Early reasoning about desires: evidence from 14- and 18-month-olds. , 1997, Developmental psychology.

[28]  E. Spelke,et al.  Origins of knowledge. , 1992, Psychological review.

[29]  H. Wellman,et al.  Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. , 2004, Child development.

[30]  Gisa Aschersleben,et al.  The link between infant attention to goal-directed action and later theory of mind abilities. , 2008, Developmental science.

[31]  D. Poulin-Dubois,et al.  Imitation of intentional actions and internal state language in infancy predict preschool theory of mind skills , 2007 .

[32]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[33]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Understanding other minds : perspectives from autism , 1994 .

[34]  S. Gelman,et al.  Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity In Cognition And Culture , 1994 .

[35]  J. Steven Reznick,et al.  Early Development of Executive Function: A Problem-Solving Framework , 1997 .

[36]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Chapter 5 – A Theory of the Role of Imitation in the Emergence of Self , 1995 .

[37]  J. Colombo,et al.  The stability of visual habituation during the first year of life. , 1987, Child development.

[38]  M. Bornstein,et al.  Continuity in mental development from infancy. , 1986, Child development.

[39]  H. Wellman,et al.  Infant social attention predicts preschool social cognition. , 2004, Developmental science.