What's domain-specific about theory of mind?

Abstract Twenty years ago, Baron-Cohen and colleagues argued that autistic performance on false belief tests was explained by a deficit in metarepresentation. Subsequent research moved from the view that the mind has a domain-general capacity for metarepresentation to the view that the mind has a domain-specific mechanism for metarepresentation of mental states per se, i.e., the theory of mind mechanism (ToMM). We argue that 20 years of data collection in lesion patients and children with autism supports a more parsimonious view closer to that of the 1985 paper. Lower-level domain-specific mechanisms—e.g., tracking gaze, joint attention—interacting with higher-level domain-general mechanisms for metarepresentation, recursion, and executive function can account for observed patterns of deficits in both autism and neurological patients. The performance of children with autism or orbitofrontal patients on ToM tests can be explained more parsimoniously by their deficits in lower-level domain-specific mechanisms for processing social information. Without proper inputs, the intact capacity for metarepresentation by itself cannot make correct ToM inferences. Children with autism have no impairment in false photograph tests because their metarepresentational capacity is intact and they have no impairment in inputs required for such tests. TPJ patients have equivalent deficits on ToM and non-ToM metarepresentational tasks, consistent with a failure in domain-general processing. If deficits on ToM tasks can result from deficits in low-level input systems or in higher-level domain-general capacities, postulating a separate ToM mechanism may have been an unnecessary theoretical move.

[1]  Henry M. Wellman,et al.  Developing understandings of mind , 2000 .

[2]  S. Baron-Cohen The autistic child's theory of mind: a case of specific developmental delay. , 1989, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[3]  G. Baird,et al.  Testing joint attention, imitation, and play as infancy precursors to language and theory of mind , 2000 .

[4]  Alan M. Leslie,et al.  Pretense, Autism, and the Theory-of-Mind Module , 1992 .

[5]  B. Pennington,et al.  Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: relationship to theory of mind. , 1991, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[6]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see , 2000, Animal Behaviour.

[7]  J. Morton,et al.  The cognitive basis of a biological disorder: autism , 1991, Trends in Neurosciences.

[8]  Dana Samson,et al.  Left temporoparietal junction is necessary for representing someone else's belief , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[9]  Kim Sterelny,et al.  From mating to mentality: evaluating evolutionary psychology , 2003 .

[10]  R Saxe,et al.  People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.

[11]  Alan M. Leslie,et al.  Domain specificity in conceptual development: Neuropsychological evidence from autism , 1992, Cognition.

[12]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  The detection of contingency and animacy from simple animations in the human brain. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[13]  Simon Baron-Cohen,et al.  Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience, 2nd ed. , 2000 .

[14]  F. Happé Autism: cognitive deficit or cognitive style? , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[15]  R. Schultz Developmental deficits in social perception in autism: the role of the amygdala and fusiform face area , 2005, International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience.

[16]  H. Wellman,et al.  Infant social attention predicts preschool social cognition. , 2004, Developmental science.

[17]  A. J. Mistlin,et al.  Social signals analyzed at the single cell level : someone is looking at me, something touched me, something moved! , 1990 .

[18]  L. Moses,et al.  Individual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind. , 2001, Child development.

[19]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Testing the domain-specificity of a theory of mind deficit in brain-injured patients: Evidence for consistent performance on non-verbal, “reality-unknown” false belief and false photograph tasks , 2007, Cognition.

[20]  M. D. Rutherford,et al.  Cognitive Underpinnings of Pretend Play in Autism , 2003, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[21]  D. Pallin Does a Normal , 2003 .

[22]  K. Sterelny The Representational Theory Of Mind , 1991 .

[23]  V. Stone Theory of mind and the evolution of social intelligence , 2005 .

[24]  B. Pennington,et al.  Executive functions in young children with autism. , 1999, Child development.

[25]  M. Wheeler,et al.  The Grain of Domains: The Evolutionary-Psychological Case Against Domain-General Cognition , 2004 .

[26]  Does the normal brain have a theory of mind? , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[27]  H. Wellman,et al.  Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. , 2001, Child development.

[28]  J. Stevenson The descent of mind: Psychological perspectives on hominid evolution , 2001 .

[29]  R. Jones,et al.  Recognition of Faux Pas by Normally Developing Children and Children with Asperger Syndrome or High-Functioning Autism , 1999, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[30]  A. Whiten,et al.  Mental evolution and development: Evidence for secondary representation in children, great apes, and other animals. , 2001 .

[31]  A. Leslie Pretending and believing: issues in the theory of ToMM , 1994, Cognition.

[32]  S. Carey,et al.  Understanding other minds: linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging. , 2004, Annual review of psychology.

[33]  神代 真里 Bidirectional communication between a Japanese monkey and a human through eye gaze and pointing , 2004 .

[34]  J Townsend,et al.  Impairment in shifting attention in autistic and cerebellar patients. , 1994, Behavioral neuroscience.

[35]  J. Haxby,et al.  Distinct representations of eye gaze and identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[36]  R. Baillargeon,et al.  Do 15-Month-Old Infants Understand False Beliefs? , 2005, Science.

[37]  David M. Sobel,et al.  Children’s understanding of the mind’s involvement in pretense: do words bend the truth? , 2002 .

[38]  R. Knight,et al.  Frontal Lobe Contributions to Theory of Mind , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[39]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  The Empathy Quotient: An Investigation of Adults with Asperger Syndrome or High Functioning Autism, and Normal Sex Differences , 2004, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[40]  F. Happé,et al.  Acquired mind-blindness following frontal lobe surgery? A single case study of impaired ‘theory of mind’ in a patient treated with stereotactic anterior capsulotomy , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[41]  E. Rolls,et al.  Face and voice expression identification in patients with emotional and behavioural changes following ventral frontal lobe damage , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[42]  D. Perrett,et al.  Neural Representation for the Perception of the Intentionality of Actions , 2000, Brain and Cognition.

[43]  A. Whiten,et al.  Reinterpreting the mentality of apes , 2003 .

[44]  A. Woodward Infants' ability to distinguish between purposeful and non-purposeful behaviors , 1999 .

[45]  Thomas Suddendorf,et al.  The Rise of the Metamind , 1997 .

[46]  J. Hodges,et al.  Theory of mind in frontotemporal dementia , 2006, Social neuroscience.

[47]  S. Ozonoff Reliability and validity of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in studies of autism. , 1995 .

[48]  J. Boucher,et al.  Unfamiliar face recognition in relatively able autistic children. , 1992, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[49]  G. Dawson,et al.  Early social attention impairments in autism: social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. , 2004, Developmental psychology.

[50]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Frontal and Temporo-Parietal Lobe Contributions to Theory of Mind: Neuropsychological Evidence from a False-Belief Task with Reduced Language and Executive Demands , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[51]  Jennie E. Pyers,et al.  Complements to cognition: a longitudinal study of the relationship between complex syntax and false-belief-understanding , 2002 .

[52]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Children with Autism Fail to Orient to Naturally Occurring Social Stimuli , 1998, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[53]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind” ? , 1985, Cognition.

[54]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  A mathematician, a physicist and a computer scientist with Asperger syndrome: Performance on folk psychology and folk physics tests , 1999 .

[55]  A. Leslie Pretense and representation: The origins of "theory of mind." , 1987 .

[56]  D. Olson,et al.  Developing theories of mind , 1988 .

[57]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Domain-specificity and theory of mind: evaluating neuropsychological evidence , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[58]  D. Povinelli,et al.  Mindblindness. An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind Simon Baron-Cohen 1995 , 1996, Trends in Neurosciences.

[59]  S. Brison The Intentional Stance , 1989 .

[60]  A. Gopnik,et al.  Why the Child's Theory of Mind Really Is a Theory , 1992 .

[61]  N. Endler,et al.  Coping with human immunodeficiency virus: Do optimists fare better? , 2002 .

[62]  Einar M. Rønquist,et al.  Foreword , 1999, Biological Psychiatry.

[63]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  The cognitive neuroscience of autism , 2004, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry.

[64]  U. Frith,et al.  The impact of extensive medial frontal lobe damage on 'Theory of Mind' and cognition. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[65]  Thomas R. Keenan Memory Span as a Predictor of False Belief Understanding , 1998 .

[66]  György Gergely,et al.  One-year-old infants use teleological representations of actions productively , 2003, Cogn. Sci..

[67]  J. Russell,et al.  What do executive factors contribute to the failure on false belief tasks by children with autism? , 1999, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[68]  A. Cowey,et al.  Sensitivity to eye gaze in prosopagnosic patients and monkeys with superior temporal sulcus ablation , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[69]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Theory of mind in patients with frontal variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer's disease: theoretical and practical implications. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[70]  Ian Apperly,et al.  False belief reasoning and the acquisition of relative clause sentences. , 2003, Child development.

[71]  D. Neary,et al.  Social cognition in frontotemporal dementia and Huntington’s disease , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[72]  V. Stone The moral dimensions of human social intelligence , 2006 .

[73]  Geraldine Dawson,et al.  Young children with autism show atypical brain responses to fearful versus neutral facial expressions of emotion. , 2004, Developmental science.

[74]  Max Coltheart,et al.  Modularity and cognition , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[75]  Philip Gerrans The theory of mind module in evolutionary psychology , 2002 .

[76]  G. Baird,et al.  A screening instrument for autism at 18 months of age: a 6-year follow-up study. , 2000, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[77]  Animacy and propulsion in infancy: Tracking, waving and reaching to self‐propelled and induced moving objects , 1999 .