Altering spatial priority maps via statistical learning of target selection and distractor filtering

The cognitive system has the capacity to learn and make use of environmental regularities - known as statistical learning (SL), including for the implicit guidance of attention. For instance, it is known that attentional selection is biased according to the spatial probability of targets; similarly, changes in distractor filtering can be triggered by the unequal spatial distribution of distractors. Open questions remain regarding the cognitive/neuronal mechanisms underlying SL of target selection and distractor filtering. Crucially, it is unclear whether the two processes rely on shared neuronal machinery, with unavoidable cross-talk, or they are fully independent, an issue that we directly addressed here. In a series of visual search experiments, participants had to discriminate a target stimulus, while ignoring a task-irrelevant salient distractor (when present). We systematically manipulated spatial probabilities of either one or the other stimulus, or both. We then measured performance to evaluate the direct effects of the applied contingent probability distribution (e.g., effects on target selection of the spatial imbalance in target occurrence across locations) as well as its indirect or "transfer" effects (e.g., effects of the same spatial imbalance on distractor filtering across locations). By this approach, we confirmed that SL of both target and distractor location implicitly bias attention. Most importantly, we described substantial indirect effects, with the unequal spatial probability of the target affecting filtering efficiency and, vice versa, the unequal spatial probability of the distractor affecting target selection efficiency across locations. The observed cross-talk demonstrates that SL of target selection and distractor filtering are instantiated via (at least partly) shared neuronal machinery, as further corroborated by strong correlations between direct and indirect effects at the level of individual participants. Our findings are compatible with the notion that both kinds of SL adjust the priority of specific locations within attentional priority maps of space.

[1]  Xin Jin,et al.  Shaping action sequences in basal ganglia circuits , 2015, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[2]  Yuhong V. Jiang,et al.  Habitual versus goal-driven attention , 2017, Cortex.

[3]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Neuronal Activity in the Lateral Intraparietal Area and Spatial Attention , 2003, Science.

[4]  R. Aslin,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article UNSUPERVISED STATISTICAL LEARNING OF HIGHER-ORDER SPATIAL STRUCTURES FROM VISUAL SCENES , 2022 .

[5]  T. Moore,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Selective Visual Attention. , 2017, Annual review of psychology.

[6]  C. Koch,et al.  Computational modelling of visual attention , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[7]  S. Yantis,et al.  Selective visual attention and perceptual coherence , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[8]  Yuhong V Jiang,et al.  Rapid acquisition but slow extinction of an attentional bias in space. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  Michael Zehetleitner,et al.  Probability cueing of distractor locations: both intertrial facilitation and statistical learning mediate interference reduction , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[10]  A. Graybiel Habits, rituals, and the evaluative brain. , 2008, Annual review of neuroscience.

[11]  G. Mangun,et al.  Signal enhancement and suppression during visual–spatial selective attention , 2010, Brain Research.

[12]  Erik D. Thiessen,et al.  What's statistical about learning? Insights from modelling statistical learning as a set of memory processes , 2017, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[13]  Britt Anderson,et al.  Spatial Probability Aids Visual Stimulus Discrimination , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[14]  Leonardo Chelazzi,et al.  The costly filtering of potential distraction: evidence for a supramodal mechanism. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[15]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Priming of pop-out: II. The role of position , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.

[16]  B. Anderson The attention habit: how reward learning shapes attentional selection , 2016, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[17]  Yoolim Hong,et al.  Implicitly learned suppression of irrelevant spatial locations , 2016, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[18]  Jon Driver,et al.  Attentional Preparation for a Lateralized Visual Distractor: Behavioral and fMRI Evidence , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  Wilfried Kunde,et al.  Location-specific target expectancies in visual search , 1999 .

[20]  A. Nobre,et al.  Large-scale Networks for Attentional Biases , 2014 .

[21]  Jillian H. Fecteau,et al.  Salience, relevance, and firing: a priority map for target selection , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[22]  M. Behrmann,et al.  Spatial probability as an attentional cue in visual search , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.

[23]  M. Paré,et al.  Temporal processing of saccade targets in parietal cortex area LIP during visual search. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  C. A. Marzi,et al.  Distribution in the visual field of the costs of voluntarily allocated attention and of the inhibitory after-effects of covert orienting , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[25]  Denis Cousineau,et al.  Error bars in within-subject designs: a comment on Baguley (2012) , 2014, Behavior research methods.

[26]  F. Chua,et al.  Capturing focused attention , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.

[27]  S. Huettel,et al.  A nexus model of the temporal–parietal junction , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[28]  S. Yantis,et al.  Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[29]  R. Remington,et al.  Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: evidence for two forms of attentional capture. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  Roger W Remington,et al.  Rapid and selective updating of the target template in visual search. , 2017, Journal of vision.

[31]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[32]  S. Tipper,et al.  Inhibitory Mechanisms of Neural and Cognitive Control: Applications to Selective Attention and Sequential Action , 1996, Brain and Cognition.

[33]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Role of the basal ganglia in the control of purposive saccadic eye movements. , 2000, Physiological reviews.

[34]  Steven J. Luck,et al.  Multiple mechanisms of visual-spatial attention: recent evidence from human electrophysiology , 1995, Behavioural Brain Research.

[35]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Predictive coding under the free-energy principle , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[36]  L. Chelazzi,et al.  Learning to Attend and to Ignore Is a Matter of Gains and Losses , 2009, Psychological science.

[37]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[38]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  OpenSesame: An open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences , 2011, Behavior Research Methods.

[39]  Marlene Behrmann,et al.  Probability Cuing of Target Location Facilitates Visual Search Implicitly in Normal Participants and Patients with Hemispatial Neglect , 2002, Psychological science.

[40]  J Miller,et al.  Components of the location probability effect in visual search tasks. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[41]  Steven J Luck,et al.  Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention , 2012, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[42]  Hyoung F. Kim,et al.  Parallel basal ganglia circuits for voluntary and automatic behaviour to reach rewards. , 2015, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[43]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[44]  Roger W Remington,et al.  Modulation of spatial attention by goals, statistical learning, and monetary reward , 2015, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[45]  Patryk A. Laurent,et al.  Value-driven attentional capture , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[46]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[47]  L. Chelazzi,et al.  Rewards teach visual selective attention , 2013, Vision Research.

[48]  R H S Carpenter,et al.  Changes in expectation consequent on experience, modeled by a simple, forgetful neural circuit. , 2006, Journal of vision.

[49]  Alexander Thomas Sack,et al.  Moving Beyond Attentional Biases: Shifting the Interhemispheric Balance between Left and Right Posterior Parietal Cortex Modulates Attentional Control Processes , 2017, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[50]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[51]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Disentangling the Role of Cortico-Basal Ganglia Loops in Top–Down and Bottom–Up Visual Attention: An Investigation of Attention Deficits in Parkinson Disease , 2015, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[52]  Ilya E. Monosov,et al.  What and Where Information in the Caudate Tail Guides Saccades to Visual Objects , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  N. Lavie,et al.  Failures to Ignore Entirely Irrelevant Distractors , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[54]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Cortical Coupling Reflects Bayesian Belief Updating in the Deployment of Spatial Attention , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[55]  L. Chelazzi,et al.  Associative knowledge controls deployment of visual selective attention , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[56]  Michael P. Kaschak,et al.  Global statistical learning in a visual search task. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[57]  Nicholas B. Turk-Browne,et al.  Complementary learning systems within the hippocampus: A neural network modeling approach to reconciling episodic memory with statistical learning , 2016, bioRxiv.

[58]  I. Cuthill,et al.  Effect size, confidence interval and statistical significance: a practical guide for biologists , 2007, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.

[59]  Ru Qi Yu,et al.  The persistence of the attentional bias to regularities in a changing environment , 2015, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[60]  H. Müller,et al.  Attentional capture by salient color singleton distractors is modulated by top-down dimensional set. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[61]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[62]  M. Chun,et al.  Contextual Cueing: Implicit Learning and Memory of Visual Context Guides Spatial Attention , 1998, Cognitive Psychology.

[63]  Lynne M Reder,et al.  The adaptive character of the attentional system: statistical sensitivity in a target localization task. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[64]  D. Robinson,et al.  Shared neural control of attentional shifts and eye movements , 1996, Nature.

[65]  Vincent Di Lollo,et al.  Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[66]  Leonardo Chelazzi,et al.  Orchestrating Proactive and Reactive Mechanisms for Filtering Distracting Information: Brain-Behavior Relationships Revealed by a Mixed-Design fMRI Study , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[67]  Jon Driver,et al.  Fortune and reversals of fortune in visual search: Reward contingencies for pop-out targets affect search efficiency and target repetition effects , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[68]  Jeannette A M Lorteije,et al.  Priority Maps Explain the Roles of Value, Attention, and Salience in Goal-Oriented Behavior , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[69]  James W Bisley,et al.  The what, where, and why of priority maps and their interactions with visual working memory , 2015, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[70]  R. Ptak The Frontoparietal Attention Network of the Human Brain , 2012, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[71]  M. Chun,et al.  Implicit, long-term spatial contextual memory. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[72]  S. Tipper The Negative Priming Effect: Inhibitory Priming by Ignored Objects , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[73]  George A. Alvarez,et al.  The role of MT and the parietal lobe in visual tracking studied with transcranial magnetic stimulation , 2010 .

[74]  Leonardo Chelazzi,et al.  Dissociable Effects of Reward on Attentional Learning: From Passive Associations to Active Monitoring , 2011, PloS one.

[75]  Antje S. Meyer,et al.  Electrophysiological Evidence of Semantic Interference in Visual Search , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[76]  Hyoung F. Kim,et al.  Basal ganglia circuits for reward value-guided behavior. , 2014, Annual review of neuroscience.

[77]  Joy J. Geng,et al.  Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews Review Re-evaluating the Role of Tpj in Attentional Control: Contextual Updating? , 2022 .

[78]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features , 1994, Memory & cognition.

[79]  Eva Belke,et al.  Top-down effects of semantic knowledge in visual search are modulated by cognitive but not perceptual load , 2008, Perception & psychophysics.

[80]  B. Balleine,et al.  Dorsal and ventral streams: The distinct role of striatal subregions in the acquisition and performance of goal-directed actions , 2014, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[81]  Maria Concetta Morrone,et al.  Temporally evolving gain mechanisms of attention in macaque area V4. , 2017, Journal of neurophysiology.

[82]  Richard Coppola,et al.  Dynamic cortical involvement in implicit anticipation during statistical learning , 2014, Neuroscience Letters.

[83]  L. Chelazzi,et al.  Altering Spatial Priority Maps via Reward-Based Learning , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[84]  S. Yantis,et al.  Visual attention: control, representation, and time course. , 1997, Annual review of psychology.

[85]  A. Hillstrom Repetition effects in visual search , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[86]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Mechanisms of directed attention in the human extrastriate cortex as revealed by functional MRI. , 1998, Science.

[87]  Gail M. Rosenbaum,et al.  Guidance of spatial attention by incidental learning and endogenous cuing. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[88]  R. Klein,et al.  Familiarity and attention: Does what we know affect what we notice? , 1995, Memory & cognition.

[89]  G. Campana,et al.  Where perception meets memory: A review of repetition priming in visual search tasks , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[90]  R. Aslin,et al.  Statistical learning of higher-order temporal structure from visual shape sequences. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[91]  T. A. Kelley,et al.  Learning to attend: effects of practice on information selection. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[92]  Erik D. Thiessen,et al.  Statistical learning of language: Theory, validity, and predictions of a statistical learning account of language acquisition , 2015 .

[93]  Michael S. A. Graziano,et al.  The inferior parietal lobule and temporoparietal junction: A network perspective , 2017, Neuropsychologia.

[94]  J. Theeuwes Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. , 2010, Acta psychologica.

[95]  Falk Huettig,et al.  When meaning matters: The temporal dynamics of semantic influences on visual attention. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[96]  Edward Awh,et al.  Statistical learning induces discrete shifts in the allocation of working memory resources. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[97]  R N Aslin,et al.  Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996, Science.

[98]  E. Keller,et al.  Saccade target selection in the superior colliculus during a visual search task. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[99]  L. Chelazzi,et al.  How motivation and reward learning modulate selective attention. , 2016, Progress in brain research.

[100]  S. Yantis,et al.  On the distinction between visual salience and stimulus-driven attentional capture. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[101]  H. Egeth,et al.  Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[102]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Perceptual and motor processing stages identified in the activity of macaque frontal eye field neurons during visual search. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[103]  Steven J Luck,et al.  Capture versus suppression of attention by salient singletons: Electrophysiological evidence for an automatic attend-to-me signal , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[104]  P. Perruchet,et al.  Implicit learning and statistical learning: one phenomenon, two approaches , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[105]  J. Duncan EPS Mid-Career Award 2004: Brain mechanisms of attention , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[106]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Attention, intention, and priority in the parietal lobe. , 2010, Annual review of neuroscience.

[107]  Emiliano Macaluso,et al.  Attention and predictions: control of spatial attention beyond the endogenous-exogenous dichotomy , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[108]  Steven Yantis,et al.  Learned Value Magnifies Salience-Based Attentional Capture , 2011, PloS one.

[109]  David Pascucci,et al.  The distracting impact of repeated visible and invisible onsets on focused attention. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[110]  N. Turk-Browne,et al.  Attention Is Spontaneously Biased Toward Regularities , 2013, Psychological science.

[111]  Yuhong V Jiang,et al.  Task specificity of attention training: the case of probability cuing , 2015, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[112]  R. Krauzlis,et al.  Superior colliculus and visual spatial attention. , 2013, Annual review of neuroscience.

[113]  Rebecca M. Todd,et al.  Implicit guidance of attention: The priority state space framework , 2017, Cortex.

[114]  A. Clark Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. , 2013, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[115]  Gernot Horstmann,et al.  Evidence for Attentional Capture by a Surprising Color Singleton in Visual Search , 2002, Psychological science.

[116]  S. Yantis,et al.  Uniqueness of abrupt visual onset in capturing attention , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[117]  Iain D Gilchrist,et al.  Target location probability effects in visual search: an effect of sequential dependencies. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[118]  J. Gottlieb From Thought to Action: The Parietal Cortex as a Bridge between Perception, Action, and Cognition , 2007, Neuron.

[119]  David Pascucci,et al.  Short-term and long-term plasticity in the visual-attention system: Evidence from habituation of attentional capture , 2016, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[120]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of spatial selective attention in areas V1, V2, and V4 of macaque visual cortex. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[121]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Integrated Bayesian models of learning and decision making for saccadic eye movements☆ , 2008, Neural Networks.

[122]  E. Newport,et al.  Science Current Directions in Psychological Statistical Learning : from Acquiring Specific Items to Forming General Rules on Behalf Of: Association for Psychological Science , 2022 .

[123]  Ben M. Crittenden,et al.  Distinct Mechanisms for Distractor Suppression and Target Facilitation , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[124]  M. Shaw,et al.  Optimal allocation of cognitive resources to spatial locations. , 1977, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[125]  J. Theeuwes Perceptual selectivity for color and form , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[126]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Attentional control during visual search: the effect of irrelevant singletons. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[127]  S. Tipper,et al.  A model of inhibitory mechanisms in selective attention. , 1994 .

[128]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Spatial Attention, Precision, and Bayesian Inference: A Study of Saccadic Response Speed , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[129]  Etienne Olivier,et al.  Contribution of the Monkey Frontal Eye Field to Covert Visual Attention , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[130]  L. Chelazzi,et al.  Visual Selective Attention and the Effects of Monetary Rewards , 2006, Psychological science.

[131]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Statistical Regularities Modulate Attentional Capture , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[132]  S. Yantis,et al.  Preparatory activity in visual cortex indexes distractor suppression during covert spatial orienting. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[133]  Paolo Martini,et al.  System identification in Priming of Pop-Out , 2010, Vision Research.

[134]  J. Duhamel,et al.  The relationship between spatial attention and saccades in the frontoparietal network of the monkey , 2011, The European journal of neuroscience.

[135]  Uri Hasson,et al.  Uncertainty in visual and auditory series is coded by modality‐general and modality‐specific neural systems , 2014, Human brain mapping.