Neural Activity Associated with the Passive Prediction of Ambiguity and Risk for Aversive Events

In economic decision making, outcomes are described in terms of risk (uncertain outcomes with certain probabilities) and ambiguity (uncertain outcomes with uncertain probabilities). Humans are more averse to ambiguity than to risk, with a distinct neural system suggested as mediating this effect. However, there has been no clear disambiguation of activity related to decisions themselves from perceptual processing of ambiguity. In a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) experiment, we contrasted ambiguity, defined as a lack of information about outcome probabilities, to risk, where outcome probabilities are known, or ignorance, where outcomes are completely unknown and unknowable. We modified previously learned pavlovian CS+ stimuli such that they became an ambiguous cue and contrasted evoked brain activity both with an unmodified predictive CS+ (risky cue), and a cue that conveyed no information about outcome probabilities (ignorance cue). Compared with risk, ambiguous cues elicited activity in posterior inferior frontal gyrus and posterior parietal cortex during outcome anticipation. Furthermore, a similar set of regions was activated when ambiguous cues were compared with ignorance cues. Thus, regions previously shown to be engaged by decisions about ambiguous rewarding outcomes are also engaged by ambiguous outcome prediction in the context of aversive outcomes. Moreover, activation in these regions was seen even when no actual decision is made. Our findings suggest that these regions subserve a general function of contextual analysis when search for hidden information during outcome anticipation is both necessary and meaningful.

[1]  J. Tanji,et al.  Role of the lateral prefrontal cortex in executive behavioral control. , 2008, Physiological reviews.

[2]  E. Rowland Theory of Games and Economic Behavior , 1946, Nature.

[3]  Nick Chater,et al.  Individual decision-making , 2019, Delivering Better Policies Through Behavioural Insights.

[4]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Modelling Geometric Deformations in Epi Time Series , 2022 .

[5]  R. J Dolan,et al.  Dissociable Amygdala and Orbitofrontal Responses during Reversal Fear Conditioning , 2022 .

[6]  S. Michael Malinconico,et al.  Decisions under Uncertainty. , 1984 .

[7]  J. Tanji,et al.  Concept-based behavioral planning and the lateral prefrontal cortex , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[8]  Jesper Andersson,et al.  Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic , 2005, NeuroImage.

[9]  R. Dolan,et al.  Emotion, Decision Making, and the Amygdala , 2008, Neuron.

[10]  A. Tversky,et al.  Who accepts Savage's axiom? , 1974 .

[11]  R. Haier,et al.  The Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory (P-FIT) of intelligence: Converging neuroimaging evidence , 2007, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[12]  P. Dayan,et al.  Reward, Motivation, and Reinforcement Learning , 2002, Neuron.

[13]  J. Neumann,et al.  Theory of games and economic behavior , 1945, 100 Years of Math Milestones.

[14]  C. Summerfield,et al.  An information theoretical approach to prefrontal executive function , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[15]  A. Lüthi,et al.  Processing of Temporal Unpredictability in Human and Animal Amygdala , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  R Turner,et al.  Optimisation of the 3D MDEFT sequence for anatomical brain imaging: technical implications at 1.5 and 3 T , 2004, NeuroImage.

[17]  Joseph E LeDoux,et al.  Human Amygdala Activation during Conditioned Fear Acquisition and Extinction: a Mixed-Trial fMRI Study , 1998, Neuron.

[18]  D. Ellsberg Decision, probability, and utility: Risk, ambiguity, and the Savage axioms , 1961 .

[19]  N. Daw,et al.  Striatal Activity Underlies Novelty-Based Choice in Humans , 2008, Neuron.

[20]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  Robert Turner,et al.  Image Distortion Correction in fMRI: A Quantitative Evaluation , 2002, NeuroImage.

[22]  Nikolaus Weiskopf,et al.  Optimal EPI parameters for reduction of susceptibility-induced BOLD sensitivity losses: A whole-brain analysis at 3 T and 1.5 T , 2006, NeuroImage.

[23]  S. Rauch,et al.  A functional MRI study of human amygdala responses to facial expressions of fear versus anger. , 2001, Emotion.

[24]  S. Becker,et al.  What Price Ambiguity? or the Role of Ambiguity in Decision-Making , 1964, Journal of Political Economy.

[25]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Reward Value Coding Distinct From Risk Attitude-Related Uncertainty Coding in Human Reward Systems , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Decisions under Uncertainty: Probabilistic Context Influences Activation of Prefrontal and Parietal Cortices , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Brain Systems Mediating Aversive Conditioning: an Event-Related fMRI Study , 1998, Neuron.

[28]  Masataka Watanabe,et al.  Integration of Cognitive and Motivational Information in the Primate Lateral Prefrontal Cortex , 2007, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[29]  Samuel M. McClure,et al.  Policy Adjustment in a Dynamic Economic Game , 2006, PloS one.

[30]  Daeyeol Lee,et al.  Functional Specialization of the Primate Frontal Cortex during Decision Making , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  Michael L. Platt,et al.  Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex , 1999, Nature.

[32]  D. Bernoulli Specimen theoriae novae de mensura sortis : translated into German and English , 1967 .

[33]  P. Whalen Fear, Vigilance, and Ambiguity , 1998 .

[34]  Colin Camerer,et al.  A framework for studying the neurobiology of value-based decision making , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[35]  G H Glover,et al.  Image‐based method for retrospective correction of physiological motion effects in fMRI: RETROICOR , 2000, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[36]  P. Glimcher,et al.  Activity in Posterior Parietal Cortex Is Correlated with the Relative Subjective Desirability of Action , 2004, Neuron.

[37]  R. Abrams,et al.  Psychological sources of ambiguity avoidance , 1986 .

[38]  J. Frank Yates,et al.  Characterization of Ambiguity in Decision Making. , 1976 .

[39]  Paul Glimcher,et al.  Physiological utility theory and the neuroeconomics of choice , 2005, Games Econ. Behav..

[40]  Evan M. Gordon,et al.  Neural Signatures of Economic Preferences for Risk and Ambiguity , 2006, Neuron.

[41]  Andrew M Colman,et al.  Size doesn't really matter: ambiguity aversion in Ellsberg urns with few balls. , 2008, Experimental psychology.

[42]  R. Deichmann,et al.  Optimized EPI for fMRI studies of the orbitofrontal cortex: compensation of susceptibility-induced gradients in the readout direction , 2007, Magnetic Resonance Materials in Physics, Biology and Medicine.

[43]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Conjunction revisited , 2005, NeuroImage.

[44]  J. Gold,et al.  The neural basis of decision making. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[45]  J. R. Larson,et al.  Exploring the external validity of a subjectively weighted utility model of decision making , 1980 .

[46]  Eric J. Johnson,et al.  Decisions Under Uncertainty: Psychological, Economic, and Neuroeconomic Explanations of Risk Preference , 2009 .

[47]  R. Rescorla,et al.  A theory of Pavlovian conditioning : Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement , 1972 .

[48]  Fred J Helmstetter,et al.  Activity in the human amygdala corresponds to early, rather than late period autonomic responses to a signal for shock. , 2007, Learning & memory.

[49]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Neural Systems Responding to Degrees of Uncertainty in Human Decision-Making , 2005, Science.