One-year-old infants use teleological representations of actions productively
暂无分享,去创建一个
György Gergely | Gergely Csibra | Szilvia Bíró | Orsolya Koós | G. Csibra | G. Gergely | S. Bíró | O. Koós | Szilvia Bíró | Orsolya Koós
[1] Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al. Perceiving, remembering, and communicating structure in events. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[2] H. Bekkering,et al. Developmental psychology: Rational imitation in preverbal infants , 2002, Nature.
[3] E. Spelke,et al. Origins of knowledge. , 1992, Psychological review.
[4] G. Csibra,et al. The teleological origins of mentalistic action explanations: A developmental hypothesis , 1998 .
[5] S. Gelman,et al. Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity In Cognition And Culture , 1994 .
[6] H. Bekkering,et al. Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[7] E. Spelke. Initial knowledge: six suggestions , 1994, Cognition.
[8] T. Wilcox. Object individuation: infants’ use of shape, size, pattern, and color , 1999, Cognition.
[9] E. Spelke,et al. Domain-specific knowledge and conceptual change , 1994 .
[10] J. Mandler. How to build a baby: II. Conceptual primitives. , 1992, Psychological review.
[11] Albert Yonas,et al. Infants' responsiveness to static-monocular depth information : a recovery from habituation approach , 1991 .
[12] F. Heider. The psychology of interpersonal relations , 1958 .
[13] S. Carey,et al. Whose gaze will infants follow? The elicitation of gaze-following in 12-month-olds , 1998 .
[14] A. Premack,et al. Causal cognition : a multidisciplinary debate , 1996 .
[15] R. Baillargeon,et al. The acquisition of physical knowledge in infancy. , 1995 .
[16] M. Tomasello,et al. Fourteen-through 18-month-old infants di eren-tially imitate intentional and accidental actions , 1998 .
[17] J. Sommerville,et al. Twelve-Month-Old Infants Interpret Action in Context , 2000, Psychological science.
[18] L. Frank. The Society for Research in Child Development , 1935 .
[19] P. Rochat,et al. Young infants' sensitivity to movement information specifying social causality , 1997 .
[20] S. Carey,et al. Infants’ Metaphysics: The Case of Numerical Identity , 1996, Cognitive Psychology.
[21] U. Frith,et al. Do triangles play tricks? Attribution of mental states to animated shapes in normal and abnormal development , 2000 .
[22] Gergely Csibra,et al. Illusory contour figures are perceived as occluding surfaces by 8‐month‐old infants , 2001 .
[23] A. Meltzoff. Understanding the Intentions of Others: Re-Enactment of Intended Acts by 18-Month-Old Children. , 1995, Developmental psychology.
[24] Renee L Baillargeon,et al. Physical reasoning in young infants: Seeking explanations for impossible events , 1994 .
[25] M. Tomasello,et al. Re-enacting intended acts: Comparing 12- and 18-month-olds , 1999 .
[26] G. Csibra,et al. Goal attribution without agency cues: the perception of ‘pure reason’ in infancy , 1999, Cognition.
[27] F. Heider,et al. An experimental study of apparent behavior , 1944 .
[28] D. Premack. The infant's theory of self-propelled objects , 1990, Cognition.
[29] S. Lea,et al. Visual Perception of Intentional Motion , 1994, Perception.
[30] György Gergely,et al. Teleological reasoning in infancy: The infant's naive theory of rational action A reply to Premack and Premack , 1997, Cognition.
[31] A. Woodward. Infants' ability to distinguish between purposeful and non-purposeful behaviors , 1999 .
[32] Frank C. Keil,et al. The growth of causal understandings of natural kinds. , 1995 .
[33] Jodie A. Baird,et al. Discerning intentions in dynamic human action , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[34] A. Leslie. Mapping the mind: ToMM, ToBY, and Agency: Core architecture and domain specificity , 1994 .
[35] Z. Nadasdy,et al. Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age , 1995, Cognition.
[36] S. Baron-Cohen,et al. Understanding other minds : perspectives from autism , 1994 .
[37] D. Dennett. The Intentional Stance. , 1987 .