The parietal cortex and saccade planning: lessons from human lesion studies

The parietal cortex is a critical interface for attention and integration of multiple sensory signals that can be used for the implementation of motor plans. Many neurons in this region exhibit strong attention-, reach-, grasp- or saccade-related activity. Here, we review human lesion studies supporting the critical role of the parietal cortex in saccade planning. Studies of patients with unilateral parietal damage and spatial neglect reveal characteristic spatially lateralized deficits of saccade programming when multiple stimuli compete for attention. However, these patients also show bilateral impairments of saccade initiation and control that are difficult to explain in the context of their lateralized deficits of visual attention. These findings are reminiscent of the deficits of oculomotor control observed in patients with Bálint's syndrome consecutive to bilateral parietal damage. We propose that some oculomotor deficits following parietal damage are compatible with a decisive role of the parietal cortex in saccade planning under conditions of sensory competition, while other deficits reflect disinhibition of low-level structures of the oculomotor network in the absence of top-down parietal modulation.

[1]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Fixation cells in monkey superior colliculus. I. Characteristics of cell discharge. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  G. Avanzini,et al.  Oculomotor disorders in cortical lesions in man: The role of unilateral neglect , 1983, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  Yiannis Aloimonos,et al.  Active vision , 2004, International Journal of Computer Vision.

[4]  W. Becker,et al.  An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli , 1979, Vision Research.

[5]  P. König,et al.  Unmasking the contribution of low-level features to the guidance of attention , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  Michael Fetter,et al.  Ocular space exploration in the dark and its relation to subjective and objective body orientation in neglect patients with parietal lesions , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  S G Lomber,et al.  Reversible visual hemineglect. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[8]  T. Paus,et al.  Cortical regions involved in eye movements, shifts of attention, and gaze perception , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[9]  Chris Rorden,et al.  The anatomy of spatial neglect based on voxelwise statistical analysis: a study of 140 patients. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[10]  Casimir J. H. Ludwig,et al.  Non-lateralised deficits in anti-saccade performance in patients with hemispatial neglect , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  J. Hoffman,et al.  The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[12]  René M. Müri,et al.  Neurophysiology and neuroanatomy of reflexive and volitional saccades as revealed by lesion studies with neurological patients and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) , 2008, Brain and Cognition.

[13]  Aleksandr R. Luria,et al.  DISORDERS OF OCULAR MOVEMENT IN A CASE OF SIMULTANAGNOSIA , 1963 .

[14]  R. Bálint Seelenlähmung des “Schauens”, optische Ataxie, räumliche Störung der Aufmerksamkeit. pp. 51–66 , 1909 .

[15]  Roman von Wartburg,et al.  Residual oculomotor and exploratory deficits in patients with recovered hemineglect , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  Matthias Niemeier,et al.  Stimulus-Driven and Voluntary Saccades Are Coded in Different Coordinate Systems , 2003, Current Biology.

[17]  C. Colby,et al.  Heterogeneity of extrastriate visual areas and multiple parietal areas in the Macaque monkey , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  Stéphanie Morand,et al.  Early event‐related cortical activity originating in the frontal eye fields and inferior parietal lobe predicts the occurrence of correct and error saccades , 2011, Human brain mapping.

[19]  D. Delis,et al.  Hemispheric specialization of memory for visual hierarchical stimuli , 1986, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  P Reinagel,et al.  Natural scene statistics at the centre of gaze. , 1999, Network.

[21]  Robert M. McPeek,et al.  Concurrent processing of saccades in visual search , 2000, Vision Research.

[22]  S. E. Black,et al.  Impaired visual search in patients with unilateral neglect: an oculographic analysis , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[23]  R. Ptak,et al.  Looking left with left neglect: The role of spatial attention when active vision selects local image features for fixation , 2009, Cortex.

[24]  Erhardt Barth,et al.  Impact of dynamic bottom-up features and top-down control on the visual exploration of moving real-world scenes in hemispatial neglect , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[25]  Walter F. Bischof,et al.  Eyeing the eyes in social scenes: Evidence for top-down control of stimulus selection in simultanagnosia , 2013, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[26]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Space and attention in parietal cortex. , 1999, Annual review of neuroscience.

[27]  Radek Ptak,et al.  Spatial neglect and the neural coding of attentional priority , 2013, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[28]  M. Behrmann,et al.  Impaired Initiation But Not Execution of Contralesional Saccades in Hemispatial Neglect , 2002, Behavioural neurology.

[29]  M. Posner,et al.  How do the parietal lobes direct covert attention? , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[30]  J. Henderson,et al.  High-level scene perception. , 1999, Annual review of psychology.

[31]  anonymous,et al.  Visual agnosia , 2012, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[32]  Kirsten A. Dalrymple,et al.  Global perception in simultanagnosia is not as simple as a game of connect-the-dots , 2009, Vision Research.

[33]  B. Breitmeyer,et al.  Mechanisms of visual attention revealed by saccadic eye movements , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  J. Dichgans,et al.  Space exploration in neglect. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[35]  R. Rafal Oculomotor Functions of the Parietal Lobe: Effects of Chronic Lesions in Humans , 2006, Cortex.

[36]  J. Sprague,et al.  Interaction of Cortex and Superior Colliculus in Mediation of Visually Guided Behavior in the Cat , 1966, Science.

[37]  T. Paus Location and function of the human frontal eye-field: A selective review , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[38]  I. Gilchrist,et al.  First Saccades Reveal Biases in Recovered Neglect , 2002, Neurocase.

[39]  Derrick J. Parkhurst,et al.  Scene content selected by active vision. , 2003, Spatial vision.

[40]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Neuroanatomical Dissections of Unilateral Visual Neglect Symptoms: ALE Meta-Analysis of Lesion-Symptom Mapping , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[41]  M. A. Steinmetz,et al.  Neuronal responses in area 7a to multiple-stimulus displays: I. neurons encode the location of the salient stimulus. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[42]  H. Karnath,et al.  The anatomy of spatial neglect , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  Giuliano Avanzini,et al.  Oculomotor Disturbances in Balint's Syndrome: Anatomoclinical Findings and Electrooculographic Analysis in a Case , 1982, Cortex.

[44]  J. Gottlieb From Thought to Action: The Parietal Cortex as a Bridge between Perception, Action, and Cognition , 2007, Neuron.

[45]  Marcel Kinsbourne,et al.  Orientational bias model of unilateral neglect: Evidence from attentional gradients within hemispace. , 2013 .

[46]  R. Ptak,et al.  A non-spatial bias favouring fixated stimuli revealed in patients with spatial neglect. , 2007, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[47]  Armin Schnider,et al.  The attention network of the human brain: Relating structural damage associated with spatial neglect to functional imaging correlates of spatial attention , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[48]  M. Farah Visual Agnosia: Disorders of Object Recognition and What They Tell Us about Normal Vision , 1990 .

[49]  Armin Schnider,et al.  The Dorsal Attention Network Mediates Orienting toward Behaviorally Relevant Stimuli in Spatial Neglect , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[50]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions , 1978, Vision Research.

[51]  Céline R. Gillebert,et al.  Spatial attention deficits in humans: The critical role of superior compared to inferior parietal lesions , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[52]  M. Rizzo,et al.  Looking but not seeing , 1987, Neurology.

[53]  A. L. I︠A︡rbus Eye Movements and Vision , 1967 .

[54]  D. Ballard,et al.  Eye guidance in natural vision: reinterpreting salience. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[55]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Spatial neglect and attention networks. , 2011, Annual review of neuroscience.

[56]  A. Berthoz,et al.  From brainstem to cortex: Computational models of saccade generation circuitry , 2005, Progress in Neurobiology.

[57]  R. Müri,et al.  Oculomotor behaviour in simultanagnosia: A longitudinal case study , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[58]  Hans-Otto Karnath,et al.  Restricted ocular exploration does not seem to explain simultanagnosia , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[59]  Keith Muir,et al.  Impairments of oculomotor control in a patient with a right temporo-parietal lesion , 2006, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[60]  J. Lynch,et al.  The parieto‐collicular pathway: anatomical location and contribution to saccade generation , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[61]  Alan Yuille,et al.  Active Vision , 2014, Computer Vision, A Reference Guide.

[62]  D. Robinson,et al.  Behavioral enhancement of visual responses in monkey cerebral cortex. I. Modulation in posterior parietal cortex related to selective visual attention. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[63]  S Ullman,et al.  Shifts in selective visual attention: towards the underlying neural circuitry. , 1985, Human neurobiology.

[64]  Tarun Kumar Dutta,et al.  Balint syndrome , 2014, Annals of Indian Academy of Neurology.

[65]  Rodger A Weddell,et al.  Subcortical modulation of spatial attention including evidence that the Sprague effect extends to man , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[66]  M. Corbetta,et al.  The Reorienting System of the Human Brain: From Environment to Theory of Mind , 2008, Neuron.

[67]  R. W. Kentridge,et al.  Independent contributions of the orienting of attention, fixation offset and bilateral stimulation on human saccadic latencies , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[68]  Kirsten A. Dalrymple,et al.  Experiencing simultanagnosia through windowed viewing of complex social scenes , 2011, Brain Research.

[69]  S. Zeki,et al.  The neurology of saccades and covert shifts in spatial attention: an event-related fMRI study. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[70]  Jacqueline Gottlieb,et al.  Simultaneous representation of saccade targets and visual onsets in monkey lateral intraparietal area. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[71]  R. Bálint Seelenlähmung des “Schauens”, optische Ataxie, räumliche Störung der Aufmerksamkeit. pp. 67–81 , 1909 .

[72]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Role of the rostral superior colliculus in active visual fixation and execution of express saccades. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[73]  C. Kennard,et al.  The anatomy of visual neglect. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[74]  F. Tomaiuolo,et al.  The anatomy of neglect without hemianopia: a key role for parietal–frontal disconnection? , 2003, Neuroreport.

[75]  R. Remington Attention and saccadic eye movements. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[76]  E A Cabanis,et al.  Location of the human posterior eye field with functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 1996, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[77]  S. Vecera,et al.  Psychoanatomical substrates of Bálint's syndrome , 2002, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[78]  R. Ptak,et al.  Cortical and subcortical anatomy of chronic spatial neglect following vascular damage , 2008, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[79]  D. Perani,et al.  The anatomy of unilateral neglect after right-hemisphere stroke lesions. A clinical/CT-scan correlation study in man , 1986, Neuropsychologia.

[80]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Neuroimaging of cognitive functions in human parietal cortex , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[81]  T Mergner,et al.  Saccadic reaction times in patients with frontal and parietal lesions. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[82]  M. Saslow Effects of components of displacement-step stimuli upon latency for saccadic eye movement. , 1967, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[83]  I. Gilchrist,et al.  Manual responses and saccades in chronic and recovered hemispatial neglect: a study using visual search , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[84]  D. Munoz,et al.  Neuronal Activity in Monkey Superior Colliculus Related to the Initiation of Saccadic Eye Movements , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[85]  R. Walker,et al.  Saccadic eye movement programming in unilateral neglect , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[86]  Hiroshi Tsukagoshi,et al.  Eye-fixation patterns in homonymous hemianopia and unilateral spatial neglect , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[87]  Stefano Ramat,et al.  What clinical disorders tell us about the neural control of saccadic eye movements. , 2007, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[88]  C. Kennard,et al.  Abnormal temporal dynamics of visual attention in spatial neglect patients , 1997, Nature.

[89]  D. Munoz,et al.  Lateral inhibitory interactions in the intermediate layers of the monkey superior colliculus. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[90]  R. Klein,et al.  A review of the evidence for a disengage deficit following parietal lobe damage , 2001, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[91]  Iain D. Gilchrist,et al.  Visual correlates of fixation selection: effects of scale and time , 2005, Vision Research.

[92]  D. Munoz,et al.  Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[93]  M. Husain,et al.  Attention, competition, and the parietal lobes: insights from Balint’s syndrome , 2009, Psychological research.

[94]  H. Deubel,et al.  Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism , 1996, Vision Research.

[95]  H. Karnath,et al.  Trunk orientation as the determining factor of the 'contralateral' deficit in the neglect syndrome and as the physical anchor of the internal representation of body orientation in space. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[96]  J. Findlay Global visual processing for saccadic eye movements , 1982, Vision Research.

[97]  S. Jackson,et al.  Dorsal Simultanagnosia: an Impairment of Visual Processing or Visual Awareness? , 2006, Cortex.

[98]  Y Agid,et al.  Cortical control of reflexive visually-guided saccades. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[99]  R. Ptak,et al.  Dynamic modulation of visual detection by auditory cues in spatial neglect , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[100]  H. Deubel,et al.  Effect of remote distractors on saccade programming: evidence for an extended fixation zone. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[101]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Attention, intention, and priority in the parietal lobe. , 2010, Annual review of neuroscience.

[102]  Patrick Dupont,et al.  Lesion evidence for the critical role of the intraparietal sulcus in spatial attention. , 2011, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[103]  G. Holmes,et al.  DISTURBANCES OF SPATIAL ORIENTATION AND VISUAL ATTENTION, WITH LOSS OF STEREOSCOPIC VISION , 1919 .

[104]  Armin Schnider,et al.  Bilateral impairment of concurrent saccade programming in hemispatial neglect , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[105]  J. Hornak,et al.  Ocular exploration in the dark by patients with visual neglect , 1992, Neuropsychologia.

[106]  D. Munoz,et al.  A neural correlate for the gap effect on saccadic reaction times in monkey. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[107]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Organization of monkey superior colliculus: enhanced visual response of superficial layer cells. , 1976, Journal of neurophysiology.

[108]  R. Walker,et al.  A model of saccade generation based on parallel processing and competitive inhibition , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[109]  J. Winn,et al.  Brain , 1878, The Lancet.

[110]  Jon Driver,et al.  Revisiting Previously Searched Locations in Visual Neglect: Role of Right Parietal and Frontal Lesions in Misjudging Old Locations as New , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[111]  N J Gandhi,et al.  Comparison of saccades perturbed by stimulation of the rostral superior colliculus, the caudal superior colliculus, and the omnipause neuron region. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[112]  R. Ptak The Frontoparietal Attention Network of the Human Brain , 2012, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[113]  B. Dosher,et al.  The role of attention in the programming of saccades , 1995, Vision Research.

[114]  Parashkev Nachev,et al.  Space and the parietal cortex , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[115]  Jon Driver,et al.  Integration of Goal- and Stimulus-Related Visual Signals Revealed by Damage to Human Parietal Cortex , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[116]  J. Gottlieb Attention, Learning, and the Value of Information , 2012, Neuron.

[117]  C. Pierrot-Deseilligny,et al.  Eye movement control by the cerebral cortex , 2004, Current opinion in neurology.

[118]  R. Müri,et al.  Visual exploration pattern in hemineglect , 2009, Psychological research.

[119]  A. L. Yarbus,et al.  Eye Movements and Vision , 1967, Springer US.

[120]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A Common Network of Functional Areas for Attention and Eye Movements , 1998, Neuron.