Rapidly acquired shape and face aftereffects are retinotopic and local in origin
暂无分享,去创建一个
David R. Badcock | Jason Bell | J. Edwin Dickinson | Renita A. Almeida | D. Badcock | J. Dickinson | Jason Bell | Hayley K. Mighall
[1] G. Westheimer,et al. Spatial location and hyperacuity: The centre/surround localization contribution function has two substrates , 1985, Vision Research.
[2] G. Kovács,et al. Adaptation duration affects the spatial selectivity of facial aftereffects , 2007, Vision Research.
[3] Rachel A Robbins,et al. Aftereffects for face attributes with different natural variability: adapter position effects and neural models. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[4] Richard M. Lipkin,et al. Adaptation across the Cortical Hierarchy: Low-Level Curve Adaptation Affects High-Level Facial-Expression Judgments , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[5] G. Rhodes,et al. The dynamics of visual adaptation to faces , 2005, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[6] J. B. Levitt,et al. Receptive fields and functional architecture of macaque V2. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.
[7] G. Rhodes,et al. The dynamics of visual adaptation to faces. , 2005, Proceedings. Biological sciences.
[8] Jason Bell,et al. Local motion effects on form in radial frequency patterns. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[9] Gunter Loffler,et al. Perception of contours and shapes: Low and intermediate stage mechanisms , 2008, Vision Research.
[10] Mark W. Greenlee,et al. Position-specific and position-invariant face aftereffects reflect the adaptation of different cortical areas , 2008, NeuroImage.
[11] J M Findlay,et al. Eye Movement Strategies Involved in Face Perception , 2013, Perception.
[12] D. Melcher. Predictive remapping of visual features precedes saccadic eye movements , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.
[13] J. Gibson. Adaptation, after-effect, and contrast in the perception of tilted lines. II. Simultaneous contrast and the areal restriction of the after-effect. , 1937 .
[14] David R. Badcock,et al. Narrow-band radial frequency shape channels revealed by sub-threshold summation , 2009, Vision Research.
[15] D. Regan,et al. Shape discrimination and the judgement of perfect symmetry: Dissociation of shape from size , 1992, Vision Research.
[16] M. Webster,et al. Adaptation to natural facial categories , 2002, Nature.
[17] Jason Bell,et al. Global shape aftereffects have a local substrate: A tilt aftereffect field. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[18] Michael L. Mack,et al. Holistic processing of faces happens at a glance , 2009, Vision Research.
[19] Gerald Westheimer,et al. Illusions in the spatial sense of the eye: Geometrical–optical illusions and the neural representation of space , 2008, Vision Research.
[20] Gunter Loffler,et al. Local and global contributions to shape discrimination , 2003, Vision Research.
[21] B. Murphy,et al. Adaptation to natural facial categories , 2022 .
[22] P. Cavanagh,et al. Retinotopy of the face aftereffect , 2008, Vision Research.
[23] Jonathan R. McDaniel,et al. Face Adaptation without a Face , 2010, Current Biology.
[24] A. O'Toole,et al. Prototype-referenced shape encoding revealed by high-level aftereffects , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.
[25] J. Gibson,et al. ADAPTATION , AFTEREFFECT AND CONTRAST IN THE PERCEPTION OF TILTED LINES , 2004 .
[26] Bernhard Schölkopf,et al. Center-surround patterns emerge as optimal predictors for human saccade targets. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[27] D. Hubel,et al. Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex , 1968, The Journal of physiology.
[28] Patrick Cavanagh,et al. The reference frame of the tilt aftereffect. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[29] G. Rhodes,et al. Orientation-Contingent Face Aftereffects and Implications for Face-Coding Mechanisms , 2004, Current Biology.
[30] T. Salthouse,et al. Determinants of eye-fixation duration. , 1980, The American journal of psychology.
[31] G. Westheimer,et al. Spatial location and hyperacuity: flank position within the centre and surround zones. , 1985, Spatial vision.
[32] T. Valentine. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology a Unified Account of the Effects of Distinctiveness, Inversion, and Race in Face Recognition , 2022 .
[33] C. Blakemore,et al. Curvature Detectors in Human Vision? , 1974, Perception.
[34] D. Mitchell,et al. Does the tilt after-effect occur in the oblique meridian? , 1976, Vision Research.
[35] D. Ferster,et al. Neural mechanisms of orientation selectivity in the visual cortex. , 2000, Annual review of neuroscience.
[36] P. Dayan,et al. Space and time in visual context , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[37] Mark W. Greenlee,et al. Saturation of the tilt aftereffect , 1987, Vision Research.
[38] P. Cavanagh,et al. The gender-specific face aftereffect is based in retinotopic not spatiotopic coordinates across several natural image transformations. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[39] E. McKone,et al. Solving the upside-down puzzle: Why do upright and inverted face aftereffects look alike? , 2010, Journal of vision.
[40] R Sekuler,et al. Letter: Tilt aftereffect following very brief exposures. , 1974, Vision research.
[41] Jeremy M. Wolfe,et al. Short test flashes produce large tilt aftereffects , 1984, Vision Research.
[42] David R. Badcock,et al. Detection of shape in radial frequency contours: Independence of local and global form information , 2007, Vision Research.
[43] P. Cavanagh,et al. A shape-contrast effect for briefly presented stimuli. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[44] D. Melcher. Spatiotopic Transfer of Visual-Form Adaptation across Saccadic Eye Movements , 2005, Current Biology.
[45] G. Rhodes,et al. The timecourse of higher-level face aftereffects , 2007, Vision Research.
[46] Christopher P Benton,et al. Effect of Photographic Negation on Face Expression Aftereffects , 2009, Perception.