Fast but Not Intuitive, Slow but Not Reflective: Decision Conflict Drives Reaction Times in Social Dilemmas

When people have the chance to help others at a cost to themselves, are cooperative decisions driven by intuition or reflection? To answer this question, recent studies have tested the relationship between reaction times (RTs) and cooperation, reporting both positive and negative correlations. To reconcile this apparent contradiction, we argue that decision conflict (rather than the use of intuition vs. reflection) drives response times, leading to an inverted-U shaped relationship between RT and cooperation. Studies 1 through 3 show that intermediate decisions take longer than both extremely selfish and extremely cooperative decisions. Studies 4 and 5 find that the conflict between self-interested and cooperative motives explains individual differences in RTs. Manipulating conflictedness causes longer RTs and more intermediate decisions, and RTs mediate the relationship between conflict and intermediate decisions. Finally, Studies 6 and 7 demonstrate that conflict is distinct from reflection by manipulating the use of intuition (vs. reflection). Experimentally promoting reliance on intuition increases cooperation, but has no effects on decision extremity or feelings of conflictedness. In sum, we provide evidence that RTs should not be interpreted as a direct proxy for the use of intuitive or reflective processes, and dissociate the effects of conflict and reflection in social decision making.

[1]  A. Evans,et al.  Elements of trust: Risk and perspective-taking , 2011 .

[2]  T. Tyebjee Response Time, Conflict, and Involvement in Brand Choice , 1979 .

[3]  A. Diederich Decision making under conflict: Decision time as a measure of conflict strength , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[4]  K. Stanovich,et al.  On the relative independence of thinking biases and cognitive ability. , 2008, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[5]  P. V. Lange,et al.  The pursuit of joint outcomes and equality in outcomes: An integrative model of social value orientation. , 1999 .

[6]  Anthony M. Evans,et al.  Outcomes and expectations in dilemmas of trust , 2014, Judgment and Decision Making.

[7]  A. Chaudhuri,et al.  Inverted-U effects generalize to the judgment of subjective properties of faces , 2008, Perception & psychophysics.

[8]  David G. Rand,et al.  Social Context and the Dynamics of Cooperative Choice , 2014 .

[9]  D. Kahneman A perspective on judgment and choice: mapping bounded rationality. , 2003, The American psychologist.

[10]  Timothy J. Wood,et al.  The Relationship Between Response Time and Diagnostic Accuracy , 2012, Academic medicine : journal of the Association of American Medical Colleges.

[11]  K. Stanovich,et al.  Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? , 2000, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[12]  David G. Rand,et al.  Human cooperation , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[13]  Glenn F. Wilson,et al.  Task decision difficulty: effects on ERPs in a same-different letter classification task , 1994, Biological Psychology.

[14]  David G. Rand,et al.  Positive Interactions Promote Public Cooperation , 2009, Science.

[15]  Valerie A. Thompson,et al.  Intuition, reason, and metacognition , 2011, Cognitive Psychology.

[16]  Jeff Joireman,et al.  Cooperation, Trust, and Antagonism , 2013, Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society.

[17]  S. Sloman The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. , 1996 .

[18]  N. Kuiper,et al.  Convergent Evidence for the Self as a Prototype , 1981 .

[19]  Ayelet Fishbach,et al.  PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES Self-Control in Action : Implicit Dispositions Toward Goals and Away From Temptations , 2006 .

[20]  Angela Y. Lee,et al.  Feeling Mixed but Not Torn: The Moderating Role of Construal Level in Mixed Emotions Appeals , 2010 .

[21]  David G. Rand,et al.  Structural Topic Models for Open‐Ended Survey Responses , 2014, American Journal of Political Science.

[22]  Jonathan Evans On the resolution of conflict in dual process theories of reasoning , 2007 .

[23]  N. McGlynn Thinking fast and slow. , 2014, Australian veterinary journal.

[24]  M. Brandt,et al.  The Unthinking or Confident Extremist? Political Extremists Are More Likely Than Moderates to Reject Experimenter-Generated Anchors , 2015, Psychological science.

[25]  Daniel Västfjäll,et al.  Intuition and cooperation reconsidered , 2013, Nature.

[26]  Brian Knutson,et al.  Saving for the future self: neural measures of future self-continuity predict temporal discounting. , 2009, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[27]  Ralph Hertwig,et al.  Time and moral judgment , 2011, Cognition.

[28]  David G. Rand,et al.  Divine intuition: cognitive style influences belief in God. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[29]  C. McClintock,et al.  Social value related response latencies: Unobtrusive evidence for individual differences in information processing , 1993 .

[30]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Dual-process theories in social psychology , 1999 .

[31]  Dawn Iacobucci,et al.  Introduction to Mediation , 2019, Mediation Theory and Practice.

[32]  K. Fiedler,et al.  Behavioral routines in decision making: the effects of novelty in task presentation and time pressure on routine maintenance and deviation , 1998 .

[33]  M. Johannesson,et al.  Rand et al. reply , 2013, Nature.

[34]  L. Skitka,et al.  Morality in everyday life , 2014, Science.

[35]  A. Evans,et al.  Survey and behavioral measurements of interpersonal trust , 2008 .

[36]  Jean-Robert Tyran,et al.  Fairness is intuitive , 2014 .

[37]  E. Fehr,et al.  Benefits of neuroeconomic modeling: New policy interventions and predictors of preference , 2014 .

[38]  David G. Rand,et al.  Reflection does not undermine self-interested prosociality , 2014, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[39]  G. Gigerenzer,et al.  Intuitive and Deliberate Judgments Are Based on Common Principles This Article Has Been Corrected. See Last Page , 2022 .

[40]  David G. Rand,et al.  Habits of Virtue: Creating Norms of Cooperation and Defection in the Laboratory , 2015, Manag. Sci..

[41]  E. H. Simpson,et al.  The Interpretation of Interaction in Contingency Tables , 1951 .

[42]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  The freezing and unfreezing of lay-inferences: Effects on impressional primacy, ethnic stereotyping, and numerical anchoring ☆ , 1983 .

[43]  E. Austin A reaction time study of responses to trait and ability emotional intelligence test items , 2009 .

[44]  G. Keren,et al.  Two Is Not Always Better Than One , 2009, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[45]  Joyce E. Berg,et al.  Trust, Reciprocity, and Social History , 1995 .

[46]  BÓ Pedrodal,et al.  Cooperation under the Shadow of the Future : Experimental Evidence from Infinitely Repeated Games , 2005 .

[47]  A. Chaudhuri,et al.  Can Anchor Models Explain Inverted-U Effects in Facial Judgments? , 2009, Perceptual and motor skills.

[48]  J. Bargh,et al.  The automated will: nonconscious activation and pursuit of behavioral goals. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[49]  A. Rubinstein Response time and decision making: An experimental study , 2013, Judgment and Decision Making.

[50]  Marika Tiggemann,et al.  Implicit approach-avoidance associations for craved food cues. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[51]  Andrew D. Engell,et al.  The Neural Bases of Cognitive Conflict and Control in Moral Judgment , 2004, Neuron.

[52]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  A comparison of sequential sampling models for two-choice reaction time. , 2004, Psychological review.

[53]  Yoella Bereby-Meyer,et al.  Honesty Requires Time (and Lack of Justifications) , 2012, Psychological science.

[54]  P. Todd,et al.  Simple Heuristics That Make Us Smart , 1999 .

[55]  Valerio Capraro,et al.  Social setting, intuition and experience in laboratory experiments interact to shape cooperative decision-making , 2015, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[56]  Anna van 't Veer,et al.  Limited capacity to lie: Cognitive load interferes with being dishonest , 2014, Judgment and Decision Making.

[57]  N. Halevy,et al.  How decisions happen: focal points and blind spots in interdependent decision making. , 2014, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[58]  A. Rubinstein Instinctive and Cognitive Reasoning: A Study of Response Times , 2006 .

[59]  A. Evans,et al.  The Psychology (and Economics) of Trust , 2009 .

[60]  N. Ambady,et al.  The effects of fear and anger facial expressions on approach- and avoidance-related behaviors. , 2005, Emotion.

[61]  Wim B. G. Liebrand,et al.  The ring measure of social values: A computerized procedure for assessing individual differences in information processing and social value orientation , 1988 .

[62]  David G. Rand,et al.  Opposing Oxytocin Effects on Intergroup Cooperative Behavior in Intuitive and Reflective Minds , 2015, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[63]  David G. Rand,et al.  Time Pressure Increases Cooperation in Competitively Framed Social Dilemmas , 2014, PloS one.

[64]  C. Dreu Time pressure and closing of the mind in negotiation , 2003 .

[65]  Ran R. Hassin,et al.  Non-conscious goal conflicts , 2011 .

[66]  Gregory R. Lockhead,et al.  Identification of Integral Stimuli. , 1977 .

[67]  Ellen Garbarino,et al.  Cognitive Effort, Affect, and Choice , 1997 .

[68]  Ying-yi Hong,et al.  Dynamic Influences of Culture on Cooperation in the Prisoner's Dilemma , 2005, Psychological science.

[69]  Giuseppe Sartori,et al.  Mentalizing in economic decision-making , 2008, Behavioural Brain Research.

[70]  R. Ratcliff Theoretical interpretations of the speed and accuracy of positive and negative responses. , 1985, Psychological review.

[71]  Nazar Akrami,et al.  Personality scale response latencies as self-schema indicators: The inverted-U effect revisited , 2007 .

[72]  Michela Sarlo,et al.  Attentional Resources Measured by Reaction Times Highlight Differences Within Pleasant and Unpleasant, High Arousing Stimuli , 2002 .

[73]  Jonathan Baron,et al.  Use of a Rasch model to predict response times to utilitarian moral dilemmas , 2012, Synthese.

[74]  A. Rangel,et al.  Multialternative drift-diffusion model predicts the relationship between visual fixations and choice in value-based decisions , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[75]  V. Capraro,et al.  Social setting, intuition, and experience in lab experiments interact to shape cooperative decision-making , 2015 .

[76]  Christina T. Fong,et al.  The effects of emotional ambivalence on creativity , 2003 .

[77]  Daeyeol Lee,et al.  Prefrontal Cortex and Impulsive Decision Making , 2011, Biological Psychiatry.

[78]  Matthias Ziegler,et al.  The Situational Eight DIAMONDS: a taxonomy of major dimensions of situation characteristics. , 2014, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[79]  Jean-Robert Tyran,et al.  Second Thoughts on Free Riding , 2013 .

[80]  Verena Utikal,et al.  Affect and fairness: Dictator games under cognitive load , 2014 .

[81]  Erik Wengström,et al.  Fast or Fair? A Study of Response Times , 2008 .

[82]  H. Zur,et al.  The effect of time pressure on risky choice behavior , 1981 .

[83]  Aldo Rustichini,et al.  Your morals might be your moods , 2006 .

[84]  D. Ariely,et al.  From thinking too little to thinking too much: a continuum of decision making. , 2011, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[85]  Sebastian Lotz,et al.  Spontaneous Giving under Structural Inequality: Intuition Promotes Cooperation in Asymmetric Social Dilemmas , 2014, PloS one.

[86]  Andrew Heathcote,et al.  An integrated model of choices and response times in absolute identification. , 2008, Psychological review.

[87]  Ernst Fehr,et al.  Rethinking fast and slow based on a critique of reaction-time reverse inference , 2015, Nature Communications.

[88]  T. Yamagishi,et al.  Is behavioral pro-sociality game-specific? Pro-social preference and expectations of pro-sociality , 2013 .

[89]  Peter Wright The harassed decision maker: Time pressures, distractions, and the use of evidence. , 1974 .

[90]  David G. Rand,et al.  Humans display a ‘cooperative phenotype’ that is domain general and temporally stable , 2014, Nature Communications.

[91]  N. Kerr,et al.  Mood effects on cooperation in small groups: Does positive mood simply lead to more cooperation? , 2000 .

[92]  M. Clark,et al.  Some Detrimental Effects of Negative Mood on Individuals' Ability to Solve Resource Dilemmas , 1991 .

[93]  Guido Ortona,et al.  Is cooperation instinctive? Evidence from the response times in a public goods game , 2011 .

[94]  P. V. Lange,et al.  The psychology of social dilemmas: A review. , 2013 .

[95]  S. Frederick Journal of Economic Perspectives—Volume 19, Number 4—Fall 2005—Pages 25–42 Cognitive Reflection and Decision Making , 2022 .

[96]  K. Stanovich,et al.  Heuristics and Biases: Individual Differences in Reasoning: Implications for the Rationality Debate? , 2002 .

[97]  Robert B. Lount The impact of positive mood on trust in interpersonal and intergroup interactions. , 2010, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[98]  Jenifer Z. Siegel,et al.  Harm to others outweighs harm to self in moral decision making , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[99]  David G. Rand,et al.  Spontaneous giving and calculated greed , 2012, Nature.

[100]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Hold Your Horses: Impulsivity, Deep Brain Stimulation, and Medication in Parkinsonism , 2007, Science.

[101]  Siegfried Dewitte,et al.  Bulletin Personality and Social Psychology Are Social Value Orientations Expressed Automatically? Decision Making in the Dictator Game on Behalf Of: Society for Personality and Social Psychology , 2022 .

[102]  J. Krueger Return of the ego--self-referent information as a filter for social prediction: comment on Karniol (2003). , 2003, Psychological review.

[103]  A. Hayes Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach , 2013 .

[104]  T. Goeschl,et al.  Giving is a Question of Time: Response Times and Contributions to a Real World Public Good , 2014 .

[105]  Dent,et al.  Cognitive Load and the Equality Heuristic: A Two-Stage Model of Resource Overconsumption in Small Groups. , 2000, Organizational behavior and human decision processes.

[106]  David G. Rand,et al.  Social heuristics shape intuitive cooperation , 2014, Nature Communications.

[107]  Jonathan Evans,et al.  Rapid responding increases belief bias: Evidence for the dual-process theory of reasoning , 2005 .

[108]  H. Markus Self-schemata and processing information about the self. , 1977 .

[109]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Empathy examined through the neural mechanisms involved in imagining how I feel versus how you feel pain , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[110]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Intuitive Prosociality , 2013 .

[111]  Andreas Glöckner,et al.  Social Value Orientation and information search in social dilemmas: An eye-tracking analysis , 2013 .

[112]  David G. Rand,et al.  It's the Thought That Counts: The Role of Intentions in Noisy Repeated Games , 2015 .

[113]  H. de Wit,et al.  Reward discounting as a measure of impulsive behavior in a psychiatric outpatient population. , 2000, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[114]  L. Skitka,et al.  Gut Reactions , 2009, Psychological science.

[115]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  of Experimental Psychology : General Anchoring and Adjustment During Social Inferences , 2012 .

[116]  Deepak Malhotra,et al.  Attributions of trust and the calculus of reciprocity , 2003 .