Fast but Not Intuitive, Slow but Not Reflective: Decision Conflict Drives Reaction Times in Social Dilemmas
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] A. Evans,et al. Elements of trust: Risk and perspective-taking , 2011 .
[2] T. Tyebjee. Response Time, Conflict, and Involvement in Brand Choice , 1979 .
[3] A. Diederich. Decision making under conflict: Decision time as a measure of conflict strength , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[4] K. Stanovich,et al. On the relative independence of thinking biases and cognitive ability. , 2008, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[5] P. V. Lange,et al. The pursuit of joint outcomes and equality in outcomes: An integrative model of social value orientation. , 1999 .
[6] Anthony M. Evans,et al. Outcomes and expectations in dilemmas of trust , 2014, Judgment and Decision Making.
[7] A. Chaudhuri,et al. Inverted-U effects generalize to the judgment of subjective properties of faces , 2008, Perception & psychophysics.
[8] David G. Rand,et al. Social Context and the Dynamics of Cooperative Choice , 2014 .
[9] D. Kahneman. A perspective on judgment and choice: mapping bounded rationality. , 2003, The American psychologist.
[10] Timothy J. Wood,et al. The Relationship Between Response Time and Diagnostic Accuracy , 2012, Academic medicine : journal of the Association of American Medical Colleges.
[11] K. Stanovich,et al. Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? , 2000, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[12] David G. Rand,et al. Human cooperation , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[13] Glenn F. Wilson,et al. Task decision difficulty: effects on ERPs in a same-different letter classification task , 1994, Biological Psychology.
[14] David G. Rand,et al. Positive Interactions Promote Public Cooperation , 2009, Science.
[15] Valerie A. Thompson,et al. Intuition, reason, and metacognition , 2011, Cognitive Psychology.
[16] Jeff Joireman,et al. Cooperation, Trust, and Antagonism , 2013, Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society.
[17] S. Sloman. The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. , 1996 .
[18] N. Kuiper,et al. Convergent Evidence for the Self as a Prototype , 1981 .
[19] Ayelet Fishbach,et al. PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES Self-Control in Action : Implicit Dispositions Toward Goals and Away From Temptations , 2006 .
[20] Angela Y. Lee,et al. Feeling Mixed but Not Torn: The Moderating Role of Construal Level in Mixed Emotions Appeals , 2010 .
[21] David G. Rand,et al. Structural Topic Models for Open‐Ended Survey Responses , 2014, American Journal of Political Science.
[22] Jonathan Evans. On the resolution of conflict in dual process theories of reasoning , 2007 .
[23] N. McGlynn. Thinking fast and slow. , 2014, Australian veterinary journal.
[24] M. Brandt,et al. The Unthinking or Confident Extremist? Political Extremists Are More Likely Than Moderates to Reject Experimenter-Generated Anchors , 2015, Psychological science.
[25] Daniel Västfjäll,et al. Intuition and cooperation reconsidered , 2013, Nature.
[26] Brian Knutson,et al. Saving for the future self: neural measures of future self-continuity predict temporal discounting. , 2009, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.
[27] Ralph Hertwig,et al. Time and moral judgment , 2011, Cognition.
[28] David G. Rand,et al. Divine intuition: cognitive style influences belief in God. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[29] C. McClintock,et al. Social value related response latencies: Unobtrusive evidence for individual differences in information processing , 1993 .
[30] S. Chaiken,et al. Dual-process theories in social psychology , 1999 .
[31] Dawn Iacobucci,et al. Introduction to Mediation , 2019, Mediation Theory and Practice.
[32] K. Fiedler,et al. Behavioral routines in decision making: the effects of novelty in task presentation and time pressure on routine maintenance and deviation , 1998 .
[33] M. Johannesson,et al. Rand et al. reply , 2013, Nature.
[34] L. Skitka,et al. Morality in everyday life , 2014, Science.
[35] A. Evans,et al. Survey and behavioral measurements of interpersonal trust , 2008 .
[36] Jean-Robert Tyran,et al. Fairness is intuitive , 2014 .
[37] E. Fehr,et al. Benefits of neuroeconomic modeling: New policy interventions and predictors of preference , 2014 .
[38] David G. Rand,et al. Reflection does not undermine self-interested prosociality , 2014, Front. Behav. Neurosci..
[39] G. Gigerenzer,et al. Intuitive and Deliberate Judgments Are Based on Common Principles This Article Has Been Corrected. See Last Page , 2022 .
[40] David G. Rand,et al. Habits of Virtue: Creating Norms of Cooperation and Defection in the Laboratory , 2015, Manag. Sci..
[41] E. H. Simpson,et al. The Interpretation of Interaction in Contingency Tables , 1951 .
[42] A. Kruglanski,et al. The freezing and unfreezing of lay-inferences: Effects on impressional primacy, ethnic stereotyping, and numerical anchoring ☆ , 1983 .
[43] E. Austin. A reaction time study of responses to trait and ability emotional intelligence test items , 2009 .
[44] G. Keren,et al. Two Is Not Always Better Than One , 2009, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.
[45] Joyce E. Berg,et al. Trust, Reciprocity, and Social History , 1995 .
[46] BÓ Pedrodal,et al. Cooperation under the Shadow of the Future : Experimental Evidence from Infinitely Repeated Games , 2005 .
[47] A. Chaudhuri,et al. Can Anchor Models Explain Inverted-U Effects in Facial Judgments? , 2009, Perceptual and motor skills.
[48] J. Bargh,et al. The automated will: nonconscious activation and pursuit of behavioral goals. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[49] A. Rubinstein. Response time and decision making: An experimental study , 2013, Judgment and Decision Making.
[50] Marika Tiggemann,et al. Implicit approach-avoidance associations for craved food cues. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.
[51] Andrew D. Engell,et al. The Neural Bases of Cognitive Conflict and Control in Moral Judgment , 2004, Neuron.
[52] Philip L. Smith,et al. A comparison of sequential sampling models for two-choice reaction time. , 2004, Psychological review.
[53] Yoella Bereby-Meyer,et al. Honesty Requires Time (and Lack of Justifications) , 2012, Psychological science.
[54] P. Todd,et al. Simple Heuristics That Make Us Smart , 1999 .
[55] Valerio Capraro,et al. Social setting, intuition and experience in laboratory experiments interact to shape cooperative decision-making , 2015, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[56] Anna van 't Veer,et al. Limited capacity to lie: Cognitive load interferes with being dishonest , 2014, Judgment and Decision Making.
[57] N. Halevy,et al. How decisions happen: focal points and blind spots in interdependent decision making. , 2014, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[58] A. Rubinstein. Instinctive and Cognitive Reasoning: A Study of Response Times , 2006 .
[59] A. Evans,et al. The Psychology (and Economics) of Trust , 2009 .
[60] N. Ambady,et al. The effects of fear and anger facial expressions on approach- and avoidance-related behaviors. , 2005, Emotion.
[61] Wim B. G. Liebrand,et al. The ring measure of social values: A computerized procedure for assessing individual differences in information processing and social value orientation , 1988 .
[62] David G. Rand,et al. Opposing Oxytocin Effects on Intergroup Cooperative Behavior in Intuitive and Reflective Minds , 2015, Neuropsychopharmacology.
[63] David G. Rand,et al. Time Pressure Increases Cooperation in Competitively Framed Social Dilemmas , 2014, PloS one.
[64] C. Dreu. Time pressure and closing of the mind in negotiation , 2003 .
[65] Ran R. Hassin,et al. Non-conscious goal conflicts , 2011 .
[66] Gregory R. Lockhead,et al. Identification of Integral Stimuli. , 1977 .
[67] Ellen Garbarino,et al. Cognitive Effort, Affect, and Choice , 1997 .
[68] Ying-yi Hong,et al. Dynamic Influences of Culture on Cooperation in the Prisoner's Dilemma , 2005, Psychological science.
[69] Giuseppe Sartori,et al. Mentalizing in economic decision-making , 2008, Behavioural Brain Research.
[70] R. Ratcliff. Theoretical interpretations of the speed and accuracy of positive and negative responses. , 1985, Psychological review.
[71] Nazar Akrami,et al. Personality scale response latencies as self-schema indicators: The inverted-U effect revisited , 2007 .
[72] Michela Sarlo,et al. Attentional Resources Measured by Reaction Times Highlight Differences Within Pleasant and Unpleasant, High Arousing Stimuli , 2002 .
[73] Jonathan Baron,et al. Use of a Rasch model to predict response times to utilitarian moral dilemmas , 2012, Synthese.
[74] A. Rangel,et al. Multialternative drift-diffusion model predicts the relationship between visual fixations and choice in value-based decisions , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[75] V. Capraro,et al. Social setting, intuition, and experience in lab experiments interact to shape cooperative decision-making , 2015 .
[76] Christina T. Fong,et al. The effects of emotional ambivalence on creativity , 2003 .
[77] Daeyeol Lee,et al. Prefrontal Cortex and Impulsive Decision Making , 2011, Biological Psychiatry.
[78] Matthias Ziegler,et al. The Situational Eight DIAMONDS: a taxonomy of major dimensions of situation characteristics. , 2014, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[79] Jean-Robert Tyran,et al. Second Thoughts on Free Riding , 2013 .
[80] Verena Utikal,et al. Affect and fairness: Dictator games under cognitive load , 2014 .
[81] Erik Wengström,et al. Fast or Fair? A Study of Response Times , 2008 .
[82] H. Zur,et al. The effect of time pressure on risky choice behavior , 1981 .
[83] Aldo Rustichini,et al. Your morals might be your moods , 2006 .
[84] D. Ariely,et al. From thinking too little to thinking too much: a continuum of decision making. , 2011, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.
[85] Sebastian Lotz,et al. Spontaneous Giving under Structural Inequality: Intuition Promotes Cooperation in Asymmetric Social Dilemmas , 2014, PloS one.
[86] Andrew Heathcote,et al. An integrated model of choices and response times in absolute identification. , 2008, Psychological review.
[87] Ernst Fehr,et al. Rethinking fast and slow based on a critique of reaction-time reverse inference , 2015, Nature Communications.
[88] T. Yamagishi,et al. Is behavioral pro-sociality game-specific? Pro-social preference and expectations of pro-sociality , 2013 .
[89] Peter Wright. The harassed decision maker: Time pressures, distractions, and the use of evidence. , 1974 .
[90] David G. Rand,et al. Humans display a ‘cooperative phenotype’ that is domain general and temporally stable , 2014, Nature Communications.
[91] N. Kerr,et al. Mood effects on cooperation in small groups: Does positive mood simply lead to more cooperation? , 2000 .
[92] M. Clark,et al. Some Detrimental Effects of Negative Mood on Individuals' Ability to Solve Resource Dilemmas , 1991 .
[93] Guido Ortona,et al. Is cooperation instinctive? Evidence from the response times in a public goods game , 2011 .
[94] P. V. Lange,et al. The psychology of social dilemmas: A review. , 2013 .
[95] S. Frederick. Journal of Economic Perspectives—Volume 19, Number 4—Fall 2005—Pages 25–42 Cognitive Reflection and Decision Making , 2022 .
[96] K. Stanovich,et al. Heuristics and Biases: Individual Differences in Reasoning: Implications for the Rationality Debate? , 2002 .
[97] Robert B. Lount. The impact of positive mood on trust in interpersonal and intergroup interactions. , 2010, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[98] Jenifer Z. Siegel,et al. Harm to others outweighs harm to self in moral decision making , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[99] David G. Rand,et al. Spontaneous giving and calculated greed , 2012, Nature.
[100] Michael J. Frank,et al. Hold Your Horses: Impulsivity, Deep Brain Stimulation, and Medication in Parkinsonism , 2007, Science.
[101] Siegfried Dewitte,et al. Bulletin Personality and Social Psychology Are Social Value Orientations Expressed Automatically? Decision Making in the Dictator Game on Behalf Of: Society for Personality and Social Psychology , 2022 .
[102] J. Krueger. Return of the ego--self-referent information as a filter for social prediction: comment on Karniol (2003). , 2003, Psychological review.
[103] A. Hayes. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach , 2013 .
[104] T. Goeschl,et al. Giving is a Question of Time: Response Times and Contributions to a Real World Public Good , 2014 .
[105] Dent,et al. Cognitive Load and the Equality Heuristic: A Two-Stage Model of Resource Overconsumption in Small Groups. , 2000, Organizational behavior and human decision processes.
[106] David G. Rand,et al. Social heuristics shape intuitive cooperation , 2014, Nature Communications.
[107] Jonathan Evans,et al. Rapid responding increases belief bias: Evidence for the dual-process theory of reasoning , 2005 .
[108] H. Markus. Self-schemata and processing information about the self. , 1977 .
[109] A. Meltzoff,et al. Empathy examined through the neural mechanisms involved in imagining how I feel versus how you feel pain , 2006, Neuropsychologia.
[110] Jason P. Mitchell,et al. Intuitive Prosociality , 2013 .
[111] Andreas Glöckner,et al. Social Value Orientation and information search in social dilemmas: An eye-tracking analysis , 2013 .
[112] David G. Rand,et al. It's the Thought That Counts: The Role of Intentions in Noisy Repeated Games , 2015 .
[113] H. de Wit,et al. Reward discounting as a measure of impulsive behavior in a psychiatric outpatient population. , 2000, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.
[114] L. Skitka,et al. Gut Reactions , 2009, Psychological science.
[115] Jason P. Mitchell,et al. of Experimental Psychology : General Anchoring and Adjustment During Social Inferences , 2012 .
[116] Deepak Malhotra,et al. Attributions of trust and the calculus of reciprocity , 2003 .