Multiple uses of visual motion. The case for stability in sensory cortex

The problem of 'readout' from sensory maps has received considerable attention recently. Specifically, many experiments in different systems have suggested that the routing of sensory signals from cortical maps can be impressively flexible. In this review, we discuss many of the experiments addressing readout of motion signals from the middle temporal area (also known as V5) in the macaque monkey. We focus on two different types of output: perceptual reports (categorical decisions, usually) and motion-guided eye movements. We specifically consider situations in which multiple-motion vectors present in the stimulus are combined, as well as those in which one or more of the vectors in the stimulus is selected for output. The results of these studies suggest that in some situations multiple motions are vector averaged, while in others multiple vectors can be maintained. Interestingly, in most of the experiments producing a single (often average) vector, the output is a movement. However, many perceptual experiments involve the simultaneous processing of multiple-stimulus motions. One prosaic explanation for this pattern of apparently discrepant results is that different downstream structures impose different rules, in parallel, on the output from sensory maps such as the one in the middle temporal area. We also specifically discuss the case of motion opponency, a specific readout rule that has been posited to explain perceptual phenomena such as the waterfall illusion (motion aftereffect). We present evidence from a recent experiment showing that an opponent step must occur downstream from the middle temporal area itself. This observation is consistent with our proposal that significant processing need occur downstream from sensory structures. If a single output is to be used for multiple purposes, often at once, this necessitates a degree of task invariance on the sensory information present even at a relatively high level of cortical processing.

[1]  Andrew T. Smith,et al.  Visual detection of motion , 1994 .

[2]  A. Norcia,et al.  Abnormal acuity development in infantile esotropia. , 1988, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[3]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Mutual repulsion between moving visual targets. , 1979, Science.

[4]  R. Andersen,et al.  Mechanisms of Heading Perception in Primate Visual Cortex , 1996, Science.

[5]  Aijaz A. Baloch,et al.  Neural model of first-order and second-order motion perception and magnocellular dynamics. , 1999, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[6]  W. Newsome,et al.  Microstimulation in visual area MT: effects on direction discrimination performance , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[7]  T. Albright,et al.  Image Segmentation Cues in Motion Processing: Implications for Modularity in Vision , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[8]  P Cavanagh,et al.  Attention-based motion perception. , 1992, Science.

[9]  Robert Jefferson Snowden,et al.  Motions in orthogonal directions are mutually suppressive , 1989 .

[10]  T. J. Sharkey,et al.  Measuring Attention Using Induced Motion , 1989, Perception.

[11]  R. Blake,et al.  Another means for measuring the motion aftereffect , 1993, Vision Research.

[12]  T. Birkhead,et al.  Extra-pair copulation and sperm competition in the zebra finch , 1988, Nature.

[13]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Monocular mechanisms determine plaid motion coherence , 1996, Visual Neuroscience.

[14]  J. Kaas,et al.  A representation of the visual field in the caudal third of the middle tempral gyrus of the owl monkey (Aotus trivirgatus). , 1971, Brain research.

[15]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Visual Motion Sensation Yielded by Non-visually Driven Attention , 1997, Vision Research.

[16]  Yves Burnod,et al.  Bayesian inference in populations of cortical neurons: a model of motion integration and segmentation in area MT , 1999, Biological Cybernetics.

[17]  Edward H. Adelson,et al.  Phenomenal coherence of moving gratings (A) , 1980 .

[18]  P. Glimcher,et al.  Responses of intraparietal neurons to saccadic targets and visual distractors. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  W. Newsome,et al.  Microstimulation in visual area MT: effects of varying pulse amplitude and frequency , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[20]  T D Albright,et al.  Segmentation by Color Influences Responses of Motion-Sensitive Neurons in the Cortical Middle Temporal Visual Area , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  H. Komatsu,et al.  Relation of cortical areas MT and MST to pursuit eye movements. I. Localization and visual properties of neurons. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  S. Zeki Vision: The motion pathways of the visual cortex , 1991 .

[23]  J M Groh,et al.  Reading Neural Representations , 1998, Neuron.

[24]  K. H. Britten,et al.  Responses of neurons in macaque MT to stochastic motion signals , 1993, Visual Neuroscience.

[25]  K. H. Britten,et al.  Spatial Summation in the Receptive Fields of MT Neurons , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  N. Graham Visual Pattern Analyzers , 1989 .

[27]  Stefan Treue,et al.  Feature-based attention influences motion processing gain in macaque visual cortex , 1999, Nature.

[28]  D. Sparks,et al.  Spatial localization of saccade targets. I. Compensation for stimulation-induced perturbations in eye position. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  A. Parker,et al.  Sense and the single neuron: probing the physiology of perception. , 1998, Annual review of neuroscience.

[30]  T. Albright,et al.  Luminance contrast affects motion coherency in plaid patterns by acting as a depth-from-occlusion cue , 1998, Vision Research.

[31]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Attentional modulation of visual motion processing in cortical areas MT and MST , 1996, Nature.

[32]  S. Squatrito,et al.  Gaze field properties of eye position neurones in areas MST and 7a of the macaque monkey , 1996, Visual Neuroscience.

[33]  R. Andersen,et al.  Encoding of three-dimensional structure-from-motion by primate area MT neurons , 1998, Nature.

[34]  R A Andersen,et al.  Transparent motion perception as detection of unbalanced motion signals. III. Modeling , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[35]  W. Newsome,et al.  Neuronal and psychophysical sensitivity to motion signals in extrastriate area MST of the macaque monkey , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[36]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Attentional modulation of adaptation to two-component transparent motion , 1995, Vision Research.

[37]  S G Lisberger,et al.  The effect of a moving distractor on the initiation of smooth-pursuit eye movements , 1997, Visual Neuroscience.

[38]  A P Georgopoulos,et al.  Neural coding of the direction of reaching and a comparison with saccadic eye movements. , 1990, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.

[39]  T. Sassa,et al.  Phytotoxic Metabolites of Rhizoctonia solani , 1963, Nature.

[40]  J. L. Gordon,et al.  A model of the smooth pursuit eye movement system , 1986, Biological Cybernetics.

[41]  Paul W. Glimcher,et al.  Response fields of intraparietal neurons quantified with multiple saccadic targets , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[42]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Movement aftereffect of bi-vectorial transparent motion , 1994, Vision Research.

[43]  G. Orban,et al.  Lesions of the Superior Temporal Cortical Motion Areas Impair Speed Discrimination in the Macaque Monkey , 1995, The European journal of neuroscience.

[44]  S. Zeki,et al.  Response properties and receptive fields of cells in an anatomically defined region of the superior temporal sulcus in the monkey. , 1971, Brain research.

[45]  R A Andersen,et al.  Transparent motion perception as detection of unbalanced motion signals. I. Psychophysics , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[46]  L. Stone,et al.  The Barberplaid Illusion: Plaid Motion is Biased by Elongated Apertures , 1996, Vision Research.

[47]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Responses of MT and MST neurons to one and two moving objects in the receptive field. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  R. Desimone,et al.  Competitive Mechanisms Subserve Attention in Macaque Areas V2 and V4 , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  J. Movshon,et al.  Linearity and Normalization in Simple Cells of the Macaque Primary Visual Cortex , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[50]  K. H. Britten,et al.  A relationship between behavioral choice and the visual responses of neurons in macaque MT , 1996, Visual Neuroscience.

[51]  R. Andersen,et al.  Transparent motion perception as detection of unbalanced motion signals. II. Physiology , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[52]  W T Newsome,et al.  Separate signals for target selection and movement specification in the superior colliculus. , 1999, Science.

[53]  G. Mangun Neural mechanisms of visual selective attention. , 1995, Psychophysiology.

[54]  D. Perrett,et al.  Brain mechanisms of perception and memory : from neuron to behavior , 1993 .

[55]  W. Newsome,et al.  A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT) , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[56]  Carrie J. McAdams,et al.  Effects of Attention on Orientation-Tuning Functions of Single Neurons in Macaque Cortical Area V4 , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[57]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  The connections of the middle temporal visual area (MT) and their relationship to a cortical hierarchy in the macaque monkey , 1983, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[58]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Preface: Cerebral Cortex Has Come of Age , 1991 .

[59]  Leslie G. Ungerleider Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .

[60]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Cortical projections of area V2 in the macaque. , 1997, Cerebral cortex.

[61]  Neuronal activity in MST and STPp, but not MT changes systematically with stimulus‐independent decisions , 1996, Neuroreport.

[62]  J. Movshon,et al.  A computational analysis of the relationship between neuronal and behavioral responses to visual motion , 1996, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[63]  O Braddick,et al.  Local and Global Representations of Velocity: Transparency, Opponency, and Global Direction Perception , 1997, Perception.

[64]  H. Komatsu,et al.  Modulation of pursuit eye movements by stimulation of cortical areas MT and MST. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[65]  J. van Santen,et al.  Elaborated Reichardt detectors. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[66]  H. Komatsu,et al.  Relation of cortical areas MT and MST to pursuit eye movements. II. Differentiation of retinal from extraretinal inputs. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[67]  A. Treisman,et al.  Voluntary Attention Modulates fMRI Activity in Human MT–MST , 1997, Neuron.

[68]  S G Lisberger,et al.  Vector Averaging for Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements Initiated by Two Moving Targets in Monkeys , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[69]  E. Adelson,et al.  The analysis of moving visual patterns , 1985 .

[70]  G H Recanzone,et al.  Shift in smooth pursuit initiation and MT and MST neuronal activity under different stimulus conditions. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[71]  D. J. Felleman,et al.  Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. , 1991, Cerebral cortex.

[72]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Submillimeter functional localization in human striate cortex using BOLD contrast at 4 Tesla: Implications for the vascular point‐spread function , 1999, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[73]  I. THE ATTENTION SYSTEM OF THE HUMAN BRAIN , 2002 .

[74]  W. Newsome,et al.  Deficits in visual motion processing following ibotenic acid lesions of the middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[75]  R. Mansfield,et al.  Analysis of visual behavior , 1982 .

[76]  Thomas D. Albright,et al.  Neural correlates of perceptual motion coherence , 1992, Nature.

[77]  L. P. O'Keefe,et al.  Processing of first- and second-order motion signals by neurons in area MT of the macaque monkey , 1998, Visual Neuroscience.

[78]  J. Movshon,et al.  The analysis of visual motion: a comparison of neuronal and psychophysical performance , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[79]  S. Petersen,et al.  Direction-specific adaptation in area MT of the owl monkey , 1985, Brain Research.

[80]  G. Shulman Attentional Effects on Adaptation of Rotary Motion in the Plane , 1993, Perception.

[81]  G. Orban,et al.  Segregation of spatially superimposed optic flow components. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[82]  S. Lisberger,et al.  Attention and target selection for smooth pursuit eye movements , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[83]  W. Merigan,et al.  Motion perception following lesions of the superior temporal sulcus in the monkey. , 1994, Cerebral cortex.

[84]  K. Hoffmann,et al.  Eye position effects in monkey cortex. I. Visual and pursuit-related activity in extrastriate areas MT and MST. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[85]  G. Mather The Movement Aftereffect and a Distribution-Shift Model for Coding the Direction of Visual Movement , 1980, Perception.

[86]  N. Sutherland Figural After-Effects and Apparent Size , 1961 .

[87]  J. Gibson,et al.  Motion parallax as a determinant of perceived depth. , 1959, Journal of experimental psychology.

[88]  H. Barlow,et al.  Evidence for a Physiological Explanation of the Waterfall Phenomenon and Figural After-effects , 1963, Nature.

[89]  Chuan Yi Tang,et al.  A 2.|E|-Bit Distributed Algorithm for the Directed Euler Trail Problem , 1993, Inf. Process. Lett..

[90]  Guy A. Orban,et al.  Segregation of spatially superimposed optic flow components. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[91]  E. DeYoe,et al.  Graded effects of spatial and featural attention on human area MT and associated motion processing areas. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[92]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Motion capture of luminance stimuli by equiluminous color gratings and by attentive tracking , 1994, Vision Research.

[93]  G J Andersen Perception of three-dimensional structure from optic flow without locally smooth velocity. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[94]  R. Desimone,et al.  Columnar organization of directionally selective cells in visual area MT of the macaque. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.

[95]  Henri Korn,et al.  Role of medullary networks and postsynaptic membrane properties in regulating Mauthner cell responsiveness to sensory excitation. , 1991, Brain, behavior and evolution.

[96]  T D Albright,et al.  Form-cue invariant motion processing in primate visual cortex. , 1992, Science.

[97]  J J Koenderink,et al.  Perception of Movement and Correlation in Stroboscopically Presented Noise Patterns , 1985, Perception.

[98]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Directional Motion Sensitivity under Transparent Motion Conditions , 1996, Vision Research.

[99]  H. Wilson,et al.  A psychophysically motivated model for two-dimensional motion perception , 1992, Visual Neuroscience.

[100]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  The Motion Aftereffect:A Modern Perspective , 1998 .

[101]  O. Braddick,et al.  Integration across Directions in Dynamic Random Dot Displays: Vector Summation or Winner Take All? , 1996, Vision Research.

[102]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  A selection model for motion processing in area MT of primates , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[103]  R A Andersen,et al.  The response of area MT and V1 neurons to transparent motion , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[104]  S. McKee,et al.  Temporal coherence theory for the detection and measurement of visual motion , 1995, Vision Research.

[105]  W T Newsome,et al.  How Is a Sensory Map Read Out? Effects of Microstimulation in Visual Area MT on Saccades and Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[106]  Maninder K. Kahlon,et al.  Visual Motion Analysis for Pursuit Eye Movements in Area MT of Macaque Monkeys , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[107]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Coding of image contrast in central visual pathways of the macaque monkey , 1990, Vision Research.

[108]  T D Albright,et al.  Visual motion perception. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[109]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Neuronal population coding of movement direction. , 1986, Science.

[110]  E H Adelson,et al.  Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[111]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Neural correlates of a decision in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the macaque , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[112]  H. Rodman,et al.  Single-unit analysis of pattern-motion selective properties in the middle temporal visual area (MT) , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[113]  Colin Blakemore,et al.  Vision: Coding and Efficiency , 1991 .

[114]  S G Lisberger,et al.  Neuronal responses in visual areas MT and MST during smooth pursuit target selection. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[115]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  A model of neuronal responses in visual area MT , 1998, Vision Research.

[116]  C D Salzman,et al.  Neural mechanisms for forming a perceptual decision. , 1994, Science.

[117]  E. Adelson,et al.  Phenomenal coherence of moving visual patterns , 1982, Nature.

[118]  A. Chaudhuri Modulation of the motion aftereffect by selective attention , 1990, Nature.

[119]  D C Van Essen,et al.  Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. I. Selectivity for stimulus direction, speed, and orientation. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[120]  J. Kaas,et al.  Do superior colliculus projection zones in the inferior pulvinar project to MT in primates? , 1999, The European journal of neuroscience.

[121]  D J Heeger,et al.  Model for the extraction of image flow. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[122]  G. DeAngelis,et al.  Organization of Disparity-Selective Neurons in Macaque Area MT , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[123]  P. G. H. Clarke,et al.  Subjective standstill caused by the interaction of moving patterns , 1977, Vision Research.

[124]  Anthony J. Movshon,et al.  Visual Response Properties of Striate Cortical Neurons Projecting to Area MT in Macaque Monkeys , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[125]  D. Heeger,et al.  Motion Opponency in Visual Cortex , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[126]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[127]  E. Seidemann,et al.  Effect of spatial attention on the responses of area MT neurons. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[128]  Alexander M. Harner,et al.  Task-dependent influences of attention on the activation of human primary visual cortex. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[129]  Alexander Grunewald,et al.  Orthogonal motion after-effect illusion predicted by a model of cortical motion processing , 1996, Nature.