Control of aperture closure initiation during reach-to-grasp movements under manipulations of visual feedback and trunk involvement in Parkinson’s disease

The present project was aimed at investigating how two distinct and important difficulties (coordination difficulty and pronounced dependency on visual feedback) in Parkinson’s disease (PD) affect each other for the coordination between hand transport toward an object and the initiation of finger closure during reach-to-grasp movement. Subjects with PD and age-matched healthy subjects made reach-to-grasp movements to a dowel under conditions in which the target object and/or the hand were either visible or not visible. The involvement of the trunk in task performance was manipulated by positioning the target object within or beyond the participant’s outstretched arm to evaluate the effects of increasing the complexity of intersegmental coordination under different conditions related to the availability of visual feedback in subjects with PD. General kinematic characteristics of the reach-to-grasp movements of the subjects with PD were altered substantially by the removal of target object visibility. Compared with the controls, the subjects with PD considerably lengthened transport time, especially during the aperture closure period, and decreased peak velocity of wrist and trunk movement without target object visibility. Most of these differences were accentuated when the trunk was involved. In contrast, these kinematic parameters did not change depending on the visibility of the hand for both groups. The transport-aperture coordination was assessed in terms of the control law according to which the initiation of aperture closure during the reach occurred when the hand distance-to-target crossed a hand-target distance threshold for grasp initiation that is a function of peak aperture, hand velocity and acceleration, trunk velocity and acceleration, and trunk-target distance at the time of aperture closure initiation. When the hand or the target object was not visible, both groups increased the hand-target distance threshold for grasp initiation compared to its value under full visibility, implying an increase in the hand-target distance-related safety margin for grasping. The increase in the safety margin due to the absence of target object vision or the absence of hand vision was accentuated in the subjects with PD compared to that in the controls. The pronounced increase in the safety margin due to absence of target object vision for the subjects with PD was further accentuated when the trunk was involved compared to when it was not involved. The results imply that individuals with PD have significant limitations regarding neural computations required for efficient utilization of internal representations of target object location and hand motion as well as proprioceptive information about the hand to compensate for the lack of visual information during the performance of complex multisegment movements.

[1]  G. Stelmach,et al.  Control of stroke size, peak acceleration, and stroke duration in Parkinsonian handwriting , 1991 .

[2]  R Caminiti,et al.  Internal representations of movement in the cerebral cortex as revealed by the analysis of reaching. , 1992, Cerebral cortex.

[3]  J. Flanagan,et al.  Neural Correlates of Internal-Model Loading , 2006, Current Biology.

[4]  T. Flash,et al.  Kinematic analysis of upper limb trajectories in Parkinson's disease , 1992, Experimental Neurology.

[5]  M. Berkinblit,et al.  Pointing to remembered targets in 3-D space in Parkinson's disease. , 1998, Motor control.

[6]  Rachel Moroney,et al.  Increased bradykinesia in Parkinson’s disease with increased movement complexity: elbow flexion–extension movements , 2008, Journal of Computational Neuroscience.

[7]  C Dohle,et al.  Human anterior intraparietal area subserves prehension , 1998, Neurology.

[8]  G. E. Stelmach,et al.  Movement precues in planning and execution of aiming movements in Parkinson's disease , 2005, Experimental Neurology.

[9]  Charles Adler,et al.  Disruptions in the reach-to-grasp actions of Parkinson's patients , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  J. Sage,et al.  The interaction of visual and proprioceptive inputs in pointing to actual and remembered targets in Parkinson’s disease , 2001, Neuroscience.

[11]  G. Stelmach,et al.  The effect of Parkinson’s disease on the control of multi-segmental coordination , 2005, Brain and Cognition.

[12]  S Ma,et al.  Two functionally different synergies during arm reaching movements involving the trunk. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  Hansjörg Scherberger,et al.  Context-Specific Grasp Movement Representation in the Macaque Anterior Intraparietal Area , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  Richard A. Andersen,et al.  FMRI evidence for a 'parietal reach region' in the human brain , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  V. Brooks,et al.  Increased Dependence on Visual Information for Movement Control in Patients with Parkinson's disease , 1978, Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences / Journal Canadien des Sciences Neurologiques.

[16]  J. Flanagan,et al.  The reaching movements of patients with Parkinson's disease under self-determined maximal speed and visually cued conditions. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[17]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Reduction in external cues and movement sequencing in Parkinson's disease. , 1994, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[18]  G. Stelmach,et al.  Parkinsonism Reduces Coordination of Fingers, Wrist, and Arm in Fine Motor Control , 1997, Experimental Neurology.

[19]  George E. Stelmach,et al.  Spatial and temporal control of trunk-assisted prehensile actions , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  L Proteau,et al.  Determining movement onsets from temporal series. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[21]  A. Gordon,et al.  Effects of a moving target versus a temporal constraint on reach and grasp in patients with Parkinson's disease , 2008, Experimental Neurology.

[22]  J. Tanji,et al.  Transformation of a Virtual Action Plan into a Motor Plan in the Premotor Cortex , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  J. Tanji,et al.  Distinctions between dorsal and ventral premotor areas: anatomical connectivity and functional properties , 2007, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[24]  Miya K. Rand,et al.  Effect of speed manipulation on the control of aperture closure during reach-to-grasp movements , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  Thomas Wichmann,et al.  Circuits and circuit disorders of the basal ganglia. , 2007, Archives of neurology.

[26]  H. Poizner,et al.  Deficits in adaptive upper limb control in response to trunk perturbations in Parkinson’s disease , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[27]  A. Wing,et al.  Grasp size and accuracy of approach in reaching. , 1986, Journal of motor behavior.

[28]  Kenji Doya,et al.  What are the computations of the cerebellum, the basal ganglia and the cerebral cortex? , 1999, Neural Networks.

[29]  G. Stelmach,et al.  Trunk-Assisted Prehension: Specification of Body Segments With Imposed Temporal Constraints , 2000, Journal of motor behavior.

[30]  Emiliano Brunamonti,et al.  Reaching in Depth: Hand Position Dominates over Binocular Eye Position in the Rostral Superior Parietal Lobule , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  Dottie M. Clower,et al.  Basal ganglia and cerebellar inputs to 'AIP'. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[32]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Effects of human cerebellar thalamus disruption on adaptive control of reaching. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[33]  M. Kawato,et al.  Reorganization of Brain Activity for Multiple Internal Models After Short But Intensive Training , 2007, Cortex.

[34]  M. Gentilucci,et al.  The control of an action in Parkinson’s disease , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  Natalia Dounskaia,et al.  Disruptions in joint control during drawing arm movements in Parkinson’s disease , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  U. Castiello,et al.  The reach-to-grasp movement in Parkinson’s disease: response to a simultaneous perturbation of object position and object size , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[37]  Dora E Angelaki,et al.  Neural Correlates of Forward and Inverse Models for Eye Movements: Evidence from Three-Dimensional Kinematics , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  Accepted April,et al.  Factors affecting higher-order movement planning: a kinematic analysis of human prehension , 1991 .

[39]  G. Luppino,et al.  Parietofrontal Circuits for Action and Space Perception in the Macaque Monkey , 2001, NeuroImage.

[40]  Kae Nakamura,et al.  Neuronal activity in medial frontal cortex during learning of sequential procedures. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  Giulio Tononi,et al.  Evaluating the role of prefrontal and parietal cortices in memory-guided response with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[42]  E. Hoshi Functional specialization within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: A review of anatomical and physiological studies of non-human primates , 2006, Neuroscience Research.

[43]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The organization of the cortical motor system: new concepts. , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[44]  Nikolaus R. McFarland,et al.  Thalamic Relay Nuclei of the Basal Ganglia Form Both Reciprocal and Nonreciprocal Cortical Connections, Linking Multiple Frontal Cortical Areas , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  C. Gielen,et al.  Differential progression of proprioceptive and visual information processing deficits in Parkinson's disease , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[46]  G. Stelmach,et al.  Effects of increased stroke number on sequential arm movements in Parkinson's disease subjects. , 1999, Parkinsonism & related disorders.

[47]  Marco Davare,et al.  Ventral premotor to primary motor cortical interactions during object-driven grasp in humans , 2009, Cortex.

[48]  P. Brown,et al.  Different functional loops between cerebral cortex and the subthalmic area in Parkinson's disease. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[49]  J. Tanji,et al.  Both supplementary and presupplementary motor areas are crucial for the temporal organization of multiple movements. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[50]  U. Castiello,et al.  The reach-to-grasp movement in Parkinson’s disease before and after dopaminergic medication , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[51]  G. Stelmach,et al.  Segment difficulty in two-stroke movements in patients with Parkinson’s disease , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[52]  C. Marsden,et al.  Simple and complex movements off and on treatment in patients with Parkinson's disease. , 1987, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[53]  Yury P. Shimansky,et al.  Role of vision in aperture closure control during reach-to-grasp movements , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[54]  Luis F. Schettino,et al.  Hand preshaping in Parkinson’s disease: effects of visual feedback and medication state , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[55]  P. Strick,et al.  Multiple output channels in the basal ganglia. , 1993, Science.

[56]  Thomas Wichmann,et al.  Pathophysiology of Parkinsonism , 2008, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[57]  A. G. Feldman,et al.  Deficits in rapid adjustments of movements according to task constraints in Parkinson's disease , 2004, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[58]  A. Cools,et al.  Evidence for lateral premotor and parietal overactivity in Parkinson's disease during sequential and bimanual movements. A PET study. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[59]  G. Stelmach,et al.  Altered coordination patterns in parkinsonian patients during trunk-assisted prehension. , 2006, Parkinsonism & related disorders.

[60]  R. Marteniuk,et al.  Contributions of gait and trunk movements to prehension: perspectives from world- and body-centered coordinates. , 2001, Motor control.

[61]  F. W. Cody,et al.  Proprioceptive control of wrist movements in Parkinson's disease. Reduced muscle vibration-induced errors. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[62]  G. E. Stelmach,et al.  Prehension with trunk assisted reaching , 1996, Behavioural Brain Research.

[63]  Miya K. Rand,et al.  Quantitative model of transport-aperture coordination during reach-to-grasp movements , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[64]  D. O'Boyle,et al.  Proprioceptive regulation of voluntary ankle movements, demonstrated using muscle vibration, is impaired by Parkinson’s disease , 1999, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[65]  R. Andersen,et al.  Intentional maps in posterior parietal cortex. , 2002, Annual review of neuroscience.

[66]  A. G. Feldman,et al.  The timing of arm-trunk coordination is deficient and vision-dependent in Parkinson's patients during reaching movements , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[67]  A. Korczyn,et al.  VISUAL CONTROL OF ARM MOVEMENT IN PARKINSON'S DISEASE , 1996 .

[68]  S. R. Jackson,et al.  The control of bimanual reach-to-grasp movements in hemiparkinsonian patients , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[69]  K. Flowers Visual "closed-loop" and "open-loop" characteristics of voluntary movement in patients with Parkinsonism and intention tremor. , 1976, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[70]  J. R. Bloedel,et al.  Control of aperture closure during reach-to-grasp movements in parkinson’s disease , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[71]  P. Strick,et al.  Basal ganglia and cerebellar loops: motor and cognitive circuits , 2000, Brain Research Reviews.

[72]  Luis F. Schettino,et al.  Deficits in the evolution of hand preshaping in Parkinson’s disease , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[73]  S. Haber The primate basal ganglia: parallel and integrative networks , 2003, Journal of Chemical Neuroanatomy.

[74]  S. Jackson,et al.  Are non-relevant objects represented in working memory? The effect of non-target objects on reach and grasp kinematics , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[75]  G E Stelmach,et al.  Stability of reach-to-grasp movement patterns in Parkinson's disease. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[76]  T. Schenk,et al.  Effects of deep brain stimulation on prehensile movements in PD patients are less pronounced when external timing cues are provided , 2003, Neuropsychologia.