Comparison of sensitivity to color changes in natural and phase-scrambled scenes.

Traditionally, thresholds for detecting photometric changes have been measured by using stimuli such as disks or gratings and accounted for in terms of relatively low-level mechanisms in the visual pathway. Therefore one might not expect the higher-order structures that characterize natural scenes to influence thresholds for detecting uniform photometric changes. We compared thresholds for detecting uniform photometric changes for natural and phase-scrambled versions of images of natural scenes. The chromaticity and luminance of every pixel was represented as a vector in a modified version of the MacLeod-Boynton color space and was translated, rotated, or compressed within that color space. Thresholds for all types of transformation were significantly lower in the raw compared with phase-scrambled scenes, and we attribute this to the influence of higher-order structure.

[1]  Erik Reinhard,et al.  Color Transfer between Images , 2001, IEEE Computer Graphics and Applications.

[2]  Mitchell G. A. Thomson,et al.  Higher-order structure in natural scenes , 1999 .

[3]  H E Smithson,et al.  Sensory, computational and cognitive components of human colour constancy , 2005, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[4]  D. Tolhurst,et al.  Spatiochromatic Properties of Natural Images and Human Vision , 2002, Current Biology.

[5]  J. Nelson,et al.  Orientation-selective inhibition from beyond the classic visual receptive field , 1978, Brain Research.

[6]  Frederick A A Kingdom,et al.  Spatiochromatic statistics of natural scenes: first- and second-order information and their correlational structure. , 2005, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[7]  M. B. Mandler,et al.  Mechanisms of simultaneous color induction. , 1986, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[8]  W. McIlhagga,et al.  Detection mechanisms in L-, M-, and S-cone contrast space. , 1993, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[9]  Arthur Bradley,et al.  Orientation and spatial frequency selectivity of adaptation to color and luminance gratings , 1988, Vision Research.

[10]  L N Piotrowski,et al.  A Demonstration of the Visual Importance and Flexibility of Spatial-Frequency Amplitude and Phase , 1982, Perception.

[11]  J. Movshon,et al.  Nature and interaction of signals from the receptive field center and surround in macaque V1 neurons. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  H. Jones,et al.  Visual cortical mechanisms detecting focal orientation discontinuities , 1995, Nature.

[13]  Chao-Yi Li,et al.  Clustered Organization of Neurons with Similar Extra-Receptive Field Properties in the Primary Visual Cortex , 2002, Neuron.

[14]  Alex R. Wade,et al.  Long-term deprivation affects visual perception and cortex , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex , 1968, The Journal of physiology.

[16]  D. Foster Does colour constancy exist? , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[17]  Bruno A Olshausen,et al.  Sparse coding of sensory inputs , 2004, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[18]  A. Gilchrist,et al.  Relative luminance is not derived from absolute luminance , 1991 .

[19]  C Blakemore,et al.  On the existence of neurones in the human visual system selectively sensitive to the orientation and size of retinal images , 1969, The Journal of physiology.

[20]  K. Mullen,et al.  Postreceptoral chromatic detection mechanisms revealed by noise masking in three-dimensional cone contrast space. , 1997, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[21]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  Neuronal responses to static texture patterns in area V1 of the alert macaque monkey. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  Nicolai Petkov,et al.  Contour and boundary detection improved by surround suppression of texture edges , 2004, Image Vis. Comput..

[23]  Kinjiro Amano,et al.  Color constancy in natural scenes with and without an explicit illuminant cue , 2006, Visual Neuroscience.

[24]  Edward H. Adelson,et al.  Recovering intrinsic images from a single image , 2005, IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence.

[25]  Eero P. Simoncelli Statistical models for images: compression, restoration and synthesis , 1997, Conference Record of the Thirty-First Asilomar Conference on Signals, Systems and Computers (Cat. No.97CB36136).

[26]  J. M. Rubin,et al.  Color vision and image intensities: When are changes material? , 1982, Biological Cybernetics.

[27]  D. Proffitt,et al.  Visual-motor recalibration in geographical slant perception. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[28]  J. Pokorny,et al.  Spectral sensitivity of the foveal cone photopigments between 400 and 500 nm , 1975, Vision Research.

[29]  K. Mullen,et al.  Evidence for the stochastic independence of the blue-yellow, red-green and luminance detection mechanisms revealed by subthreshold summation , 1999, Vision Research.

[30]  J. B. Levitt,et al.  Contrast dependence of contextual effects in primate visual cortex , 1997, nature.

[31]  D. Ferster Spatially opponent excitation and inhibition in simple cells of the cat visual cortex , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[32]  David J. Field,et al.  Sparse coding with an overcomplete basis set: A strategy employed by V1? , 1997, Vision Research.

[33]  A Bradley,et al.  Contrast dependence and mechanisms of masking interactions among chromatic and luminance gratings. , 1988, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[34]  D J Field,et al.  Relations between the statistics of natural images and the response properties of cortical cells. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[35]  H Barlow,et al.  Redundancy reduction revisited , 2001, Network.

[36]  D. Brainard,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article LIGHTNESS CONSTANCY: A Direct Test of the Illumination-Estimation Hypothesis , 2022 .

[37]  David J. Field,et al.  What Is the Goal of Sensory Coding? , 1994, Neural Computation.

[38]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Lateral inhibition between orientation detectors in the cat's visual cortex , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  Ben Willmore,et al.  The Receptive-Field Organization of Simple Cells in Primary Visual Cortex of Ferrets under Natural Scene Stimulation , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[40]  D. Tolhurst,et al.  Amplitude spectra of natural images , 1992 .

[41]  D. Ruderman,et al.  Statistics of cone responses to natural images: implications for visual coding , 1998 .

[42]  Qasim Zaidi,et al.  Color constancy in a rough world , 2001 .

[43]  Nicolai Petkov,et al.  Suppression of contour perception by band-limited noise and its relation to nonclassical receptive field inhibition , 2003, Biological cybernetics.

[44]  Victor A. F. Lamme,et al.  Contextual Modulation in Primary Visual Cortex , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  Donald I. A. MacLeod,et al.  Influence of scene statistics on colour constancy , 2002, Nature.

[46]  H. Barlow,et al.  Single Units and Sensation: A Neuron Doctrine for Perceptual Psychology? , 1972, Perception.

[47]  K T Mullen,et al.  Evidence for separate pathways for color and luminance detection mechanisms. , 1994, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[48]  Laurence T Maloney,et al.  Illuminant estimation as cue combination. , 2002, Journal of vision.

[49]  A. Gilchrist,et al.  An anchoring theory of lightness perception. , 1999 .

[50]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  Response modulation by texture surround in primate area V1: Correlates of “popout” under anesthesia , 1999, Visual Neuroscience.

[51]  Andriana Olmos,et al.  A biologically inspired algorithm for the recovery of shading and reflectance images , 2004 .

[52]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex , 1962, The Journal of physiology.

[53]  E Switkes,et al.  Simultaneous masking interactions between chromatic and luminance gratings. , 1983, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[54]  F A Wichmann,et al.  Ning for Helpful Comments and Suggestions. This Paper Benefited Con- Siderably from Conscientious Peer Review, and We Thank Our Reviewers the Psychometric Function: I. Fitting, Sampling, and Goodness of Fit , 2001 .

[55]  B. Wandell Color measurement and discrimination. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[56]  M. Webster,et al.  Adaptation and the color statistics of natural images , 1997, Vision Research.

[58]  A. Sillito,et al.  Surround suppression in primate V1. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[59]  M. Thomson Beats, kurtosis and visual coding , 2001, Network.

[60]  R M Boynton Implications of the minimally distinct border. , 1973, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[61]  D. Field,et al.  Does spatial invariance result from insensitivity to change? , 2016, Journal of vision.

[62]  Charles R. Johnson,et al.  Matrix analysis , 1985, Statistical Inference for Engineers and Data Scientists.