Rule-based category learning in patients with Parkinson's disease

Measures of explicit rule-based category learning are commonly used in neuropsychological evaluation of individuals with Parkinson's disease (PD) and the pattern of PD performance on these measures tends to be highly varied. We review the neuropsychological literature to clarify the manner in which PD affects the component processes of rule-based category learning and work to identify and resolve discrepancies within this literature. In particular, we address the manner in which PD and its common treatments affect the processes of rule generation, maintenance, shifting and selection. We then integrate the neuropsychological research with relevant neuroimaging and computational modeling evidence to clarify the neurobiological impact of PD on each process. Current evidence indicates that neurochemical changes associated with PD primarily disrupt rule shifting, and may disturb feedback-mediated learning processes that guide rule selection. Although surgical and pharmacological therapies remediate this deficit, it appears that the same treatments may contribute to impaired rule generation, maintenance and selection processes. These data emphasize the importance of distinguishing between the impact of PD and its common treatments when considering the neuropsychological profile of the disease.

[1]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Modulation of memory fields by dopamine Dl receptors in prefrontal cortex , 1995, Nature.

[2]  Pattern of intrusions in verbal recall: Comparison of Alzheimer's disease, Parkinson's disease, and frontal lobe dementia , 2001, Brain and Cognition.

[3]  R. Malenka,et al.  Dopaminergic modulation of neuronal excitability in the striatum and nucleus accumbens. , 2000, Annual review of neuroscience.

[4]  A. Cools,et al.  Card sorting performance in Parkinson's Disease: a comparison between acquisition and shifting performance. , 1995, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[5]  R. Lubow,et al.  Latent inhibition in humans: data, theory, and implications for schizophrenia. , 1995, Psychological bulletin.

[6]  M. Gluck,et al.  Interactive memory systems in the human brain , 2001, Nature.

[7]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Alternative strategies of categorization , 1998, Cognition.

[8]  T. Robbins,et al.  Differential effects of 6-OHDA lesions of the frontal cortex and caudate nucleus on the ability to acquire an attentional set. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[9]  J. Bargas,et al.  D 1 Receptor Activation Enhances Evoked Discharge in Neostriatal Medium Spiny Neurons by Modulating an L-Type Ca 2 1 Conductance , 1997 .

[10]  T. Robbins,et al.  Impaired set-shifting and dissociable effects on tests of spatial working memory following the dopamine D2 receptor antagonist sulpiride in human volunteers , 2004, Psychopharmacology.

[11]  F. Gregory Ashby,et al.  Chapter 25 – MULTIPLE SYSTEMS OF PERCEPTUAL CATEGORY LEARNING: THEORY AND COGNITIVE TESTS , 2005 .

[12]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Imaging Cognition II: An Empirical Review of 275 PET and fMRI Studies , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  C. Marsden,et al.  The performance on learning tasks of patients in the early stages of Parkinson's disease , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  K. J. Plasterk Louis Costa and The Journal of Clinical (and Experimental) Neuropsychology , 1998 .

[15]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. , 2002, Psychological review.

[16]  T. Robbins,et al.  Probabilistic learning and reversal deficits in patients with Parkinson’s disease or frontal or temporal lobe lesions: possible adverse effects of dopaminergic medication , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Prefrontal cortex and flexible cognitive control: rules without symbols. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[18]  O Hornykiewicz,et al.  Dopamine in thelimbic regions of the human brain: normal and abnormal. , 1977, Advances in biochemical psychopharmacology.

[19]  D. Morilak,et al.  Noradrenergic modulation of cognitive function in rat medial prefrontal cortex as measured by attentional set shifting capability , 2006, Neuroscience.

[20]  小羽 俊士,et al.  Wisconsin card sorting test , 2013 .

[21]  E. A. Berg,et al.  A simple objective technique for measuring flexibility in thinking. , 1948, The Journal of general psychology.

[22]  S. Segalowitz,et al.  The error negativity in nonmedicated and medicated patients with Parkinson’s disease , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[23]  C. Pantelis,et al.  Cognitive flexibility and complex integration in Parkinson's disease, Huntington's disease, and Schizophrenia , 1995, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[24]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Ventral–striatal/nucleus–accumbens sensitivity to prediction errors during classification learning , 2006, Human brain mapping.

[25]  S L Moody,et al.  A Model That Accounts for Activity in Primate Frontal Cortex during a Delayed Matching-to-Sample Task , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  Gregory Ashby,et al.  A neuropsychological theory of multiple systems in category learning. , 1998, Psychological review.

[27]  M. Moscovitch,et al.  Memory for temporal order and conditional associative-learning in patients with Parkinson's disease , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  T. Robbins,et al.  Enhanced or impaired cognitive function in Parkinson's disease as a function of dopaminergic medication and task demands. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[29]  M. Schwarz,et al.  Error processing in patients with Parkinson’s disease: the influence of medication state , 2008, Journal of Neural Transmission.

[30]  H. Soininen,et al.  Different error pattern of episodic and semantic memory in Alzheimer's disease and Parkinson's disease with dementia , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[31]  R. Bakay,et al.  Cognitive impairments in advanced PD without dementia , 2002, Neurology.

[32]  S. Petersen,et al.  Functional Anatomic Studies of Memory Retrieval for Auditory Words and Visual Pictures , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  A C Roberts,et al.  6-Hydroxydopamine lesions of the prefrontal cortex in monkeys enhance performance on an analog of the Wisconsin Card Sort Test: possible interactions with subcortical dopamine , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[34]  Lynette J. Tippett,et al.  Effects of focal brain lesions on visual problem-solving , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[35]  M. Gluck,et al.  Basal ganglia and dopamine contributions to probabilistic category learning , 2008, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[36]  Petra E. Pajtas,et al.  Single dose of a dopamine agonist impairs reinforcement learning in humans: Behavioral evidence from a laboratory-based measure of reward responsiveness , 2008, Psychopharmacology.

[37]  Mark K. Johansen,et al.  Exemplar-based accounts of "multiple-system" phenomena in perceptual categorization. , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[38]  R. Reitan Verbal Problem Solving as Related to Cerebral Damage , 1972, Perceptual and motor skills.

[39]  Giulio Tononi,et al.  Direct evidence for a prefrontal contribution to the control of proactive interference in verbal working memory , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[40]  Maddox Wt,et al.  Quantitative modeling of visual attention processes in patients with Parkinson's disease: effects of stimulus integrality on selective attention and dimensional integration. , 1999 .

[41]  A. Benton,et al.  Frontal Lobe Function and Dysfunction , 1991 .

[42]  Tim Shallice,et al.  Generation and recognition of abstract rules in different frontal lobe subgroups , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  K. Flowers,et al.  The effect of Parkinson's disease on the ability to maintain a mental set. , 1985, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[44]  O. Hornykiewicz,et al.  Increase of Preproenkephalin mRNA Levels in the Putamen of Parkinson Disease Patients with Levodopa‐Induced Dyskinesias , 2002, Journal of neuropathology and experimental neurology.

[45]  G. Fénelon,et al.  Neuropsychological prediction of dementia in Parkinson’s disease , 1998, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[46]  J. Kwentus,et al.  Modified wisconsin sorting test in elderly normal, depressed and demented patients , 1988 .

[47]  D L Price,et al.  Parkinson's disease , 1985, Neurology.

[48]  A. Tröster Memory in neurodegenerative disease : biological, cognitive, and clinical perspectives , 1998 .

[49]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Selective and divided attention during visual discriminations of shape, color, and speed: functional anatomy by positron emission tomography , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[50]  B. Scatton,et al.  Cortical modulation of striatal function , 1982, Brain Research.

[51]  J. Harris,et al.  Irrelevant peripheral visual stimuli impair manual reaction times in Parkinson's disease , 1997, Vision Research.

[52]  R. O’Reilly,et al.  Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience: Understanding the Mind by Simulating the Brain , 2000 .

[53]  Jennifer A. Mangels,et al.  A Neostriatal Habit Learning System in Humans , 1996, Science.

[54]  David Zipser,et al.  Recurrent Network Model of the Neural Mechanism of Short-Term Active Memory , 1991, Neural Computation.

[55]  Marcia C. Smith,et al.  A Comparison of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test with the Modified Card Sorting Test for Use with Older Adults , 1991 .

[56]  Antonio Incisa della Rocchetta,et al.  Classification and Recall of Pictures After Unilateral Frontal or Temporal Lobectomy , 1986, Cortex.

[57]  S. Petersen,et al.  Practice-related changes in human brain functional anatomy during nonmotor learning. , 1994, Cerebral cortex.

[58]  M. Gluck,et al.  The role of dopamine in cognitive sequence learning: evidence from Parkinson’s disease , 2005, Behavioural Brain Research.

[59]  Vinod Goel,et al.  Anatomical Segregation of Component Processes in an Inductive Inference Task , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[60]  P W Burgess,et al.  Strategy application disorder: the role of the frontal lobes in human multitasking , 2000, Psychological research.

[61]  F. Gregory Ashby,et al.  FROST: A Distributed Neurocomputational Model of Working Memory Maintenance , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[62]  Peter Ford Dominey,et al.  A Model of Corticostriatal Plasticity for Learning Oculomotor Associations and Sequences , 1995, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[63]  Dagmar Zeithamova,et al.  Computational Models Inform Clinical Science and Assessment: An Application to Category Learning in Striatal-Damaged Patients. , 2010, Journal of mathematical psychology.

[64]  D. Morilak,et al.  Chronic Treatment with Desipramine Improves Cognitive Performance of Rats in an Attentional Set-Shifting Test , 2007, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[65]  J. McGaughy,et al.  Atomoxetine reverses attentional deficits produced by noradrenergic deafferentation of medial prefrontal cortex , 2008, Psychopharmacology.

[66]  C. Marsden,et al.  'Frontal' cognitive function in patients with Parkinson's disease 'on' and 'off' levodopa. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[67]  W. T. Maddox,et al.  Characterizing rule-based category learning deficits in patients with Parkinson's disease , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[68]  David L. Strayer,et al.  Negative priming in patients with Parkinson's disease: evidence for a role of the striatum in inhibitory attentional processes. , 2002, Neuropsychology.

[69]  K. Rommelfanger,et al.  Norepinephrine: The redheaded stepchild of Parkinson's disease. , 2007, Biochemical pharmacology.

[70]  T. Robbins,et al.  Systemic sulpiride in young adult volunteers simulates the profile of cognitive deficits in Parkinson’s disease , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[71]  W T Maddox,et al.  Quantitative modeling of visual attention processes in patients with Parkinson's disease: effects of stimulus integrality on selective attention and dimensional integration. , 1999, Neuropsychology.

[72]  K. Marder,et al.  Neuropsychological characteristics of preclinical dementia in Parkinson's disease , 1995, Neurology.

[73]  W. Schultz Behavioral dopamine signals , 2007, Trends in Neurosciences.

[74]  E. T. Rolls,et al.  Responses of striatal neurons in the behaving monkey. 3. Effects of iontophoretically applied dopamine on normal responsiveness , 1984, Neuroscience.

[75]  A. Lees,et al.  Cognitive deficits in the early stages of Parkinson's disease. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[76]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Functional organization of the medial frontal cortex , 2007, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[77]  S. Channon,et al.  Cognitive strategies and hypothesis testing during discrimination learning in Parkinson's disease , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[78]  Murray Grossman,et al.  Multiple systems of category learning , 2008, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[79]  V. Brown,et al.  Mechanisms underlying attentional set-shifting inParkinsons disease , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[80]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Dynamic Dopamine Modulation in the Basal Ganglia: A Neurocomputational Account of Cognitive Deficits in Medicated and Nonmedicated Parkinsonism , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[81]  M. Canesi,et al.  Frontal dysfunction in early Parkinson's diseases , 1994, Acta neurologica Scandinavica.

[82]  J. Grafman,et al.  Concept formation and concept shifting in frontal lesion and Parkinson's disease patients assessed with the California Card Sorting Test. , 1999, Neuropsychology.

[83]  J. Aharon-Peretz,et al.  Dopamine asymmetry interacts with medication to affect cognition in Parkinson's disease , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[84]  R. Cools Dopaminergic modulation of cognitive function-implications for l-DOPA treatment in Parkinson's disease , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[85]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  On the Control of Control: The Role of Dopamine in Regulating Prefrontal Function and Working Memory , 2007 .

[86]  P. Glimcher,et al.  Midbrain Dopamine Neurons Encode a Quantitative Reward Prediction Error Signal , 2005, Neuron.

[87]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Computational perspectives on dopamine function in prefrontal cortex , 2002, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[88]  R. Elliott,et al.  Activation of Different Anterior Cingulate Foci in Association with Hypothesis Testing and Response Selection , 1998, NeuroImage.

[89]  William M. Smith,et al.  A Study of Thinking , 1956 .

[90]  C. E. Polkey,et al.  Impaired dimensional selection but intact use of reward feedback during visual discrimination learning in Parkinson's disease , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[91]  J. Grafman,et al.  ROLE OF THE RIGHT PREFRONTAL CORTEX IN ILL-STRUCTURED PLANNING , 2000, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[92]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Spared error-related potentials in mild to moderate Parkinson’s disease , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[93]  T. Robbins,et al.  Rule-abstraction Deficits Following a Basal Ganglia Lesion , 2001, Neurocase.

[94]  W. T. Maddox,et al.  Dissociating explicit and procedural-learning based systems of perceptual category learning , 2004, Behavioural Processes.

[95]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Interactions between frontal cortex and basal ganglia in working memory: A computational model , 2001, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[96]  H. Heinze,et al.  Ageing and early-stage Parkinson's disease affect separable neural mechanisms of mesolimbic reward processing. , 2007, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[97]  Elsevier Biomedical Press RESPONSES OF STRIATAL NEURONS IN THE BEHAVING MONKEY. 1. HEAD OF THE CAUDATE NUCLEUS , 1983 .

[98]  P. Grasby,et al.  Sulpiride and mnemonic function: effects of a dopamine D2 receptor antagonist on working memory, emotional memory and long-term memory in healthy volunteers , 2005, Journal of Psychopharmacology.

[99]  R. O’Reilly,et al.  A computational approach to prefrontal cortex, cognitive control and schizophrenia: recent developments and current challenges. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[100]  H J Sagar,et al.  Different effects of dopaminergic and anticholinergic therapies on cognitive and motor function in Parkinson's disease. A follow-up study of untreated patients. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[101]  S P Wise,et al.  Distributed modular architectures linking basal ganglia, cerebellum, and cerebral cortex: their role in planning and controlling action. , 1995, Cerebral cortex.

[102]  K. Marder,et al.  Memory and executive function impairment predict dementia in Parkinson's disease , 2002, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[103]  Elizabeth R. Chrastil,et al.  Sustained neural activity associated with cognitive control during temporally extended decision making. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[104]  W. Beatty,et al.  Problem Solving in Parkinson's Disease: Comparison of Performance on the Wisconsin and California Card Sorting Tests , 1990, Journal of geriatric psychiatry and neurology.

[105]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  By Carrot or by Stick: Cognitive Reinforcement Learning in Parkinsonism , 2004, Science.

[106]  R. W. Watson,et al.  A central executive deficit in patients with Parkinson's disease. , 1994, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[107]  M. Petrides,et al.  Neural Bases of Set-Shifting Deficits in Parkinson's Disease , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[108]  N. Sawamoto,et al.  Cognitive deficits and striato-frontal dopamine release in Parkinson's disease. , 2008, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[109]  Kenneth M Heilman,et al.  Visual selective attention in Parkinson's disease: dissociation of exogenous and endogenous inhibition. , 2006, Neuropsychology.

[110]  T. Robbins,et al.  Dopaminergic basis for deficits in working memory but not attentional set-shifting in Parkinson's disease , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[111]  Richard B. Ivry,et al.  Rule-Based Category Learning is Impaired in Patients with Parkinson's Disease but not in Patients with Cerebellar Disorders , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[112]  T. Robbins,et al.  The effect of dopamine depletion from the caudate nucleus of the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) on tests of prefrontal cognitive function. , 2000, Behavioral neuroscience.

[113]  M. Petrides,et al.  Wisconsin Card Sorting Revisited: Distinct Neural Circuits Participating in Different Stages of the Task Identified by Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[114]  Amanda Price Explicit category learning in Parkinson's disease: deficits related to impaired rule generation and selection processes. , 2006, Neuropsychology.

[115]  W Todd Maddox,et al.  The impact of irrelevant dimensional variation on rule-based category learning in patients with Parkinson's disease , 2005, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[116]  A. Tröster,et al.  Prodromal frontal/executive dysfunction predicts incident dementia in Parkinson’s disease , 2003, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[117]  D. Aarsland,et al.  Cognitive Predictors of Dementia in Parkinson’s Disease: A Community-Based, 4-Year Longitudinal Study , 2005, Journal of geriatric psychiatry and neurology.

[118]  H. Whitaker,et al.  Cognitive Disturbances in Parkinson's Disease , 1989, Journal of geriatric psychiatry and neurology.

[119]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Competition among multiple memory systems: converging evidence from animal and human brain studies , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[120]  Oshin Vartanian,et al.  Task constraints modulate activation in right ventral lateral prefrontal cortex , 2005, NeuroImage.

[121]  J. Cohen,et al.  Dopamine, cognitive control, and schizophrenia: the gating model. , 1999, Progress in brain research.

[122]  R. Lubow,et al.  Latent inhibition and recall/recognition of irrelevant stimuli as a function of pre-exposure duration in high and low psychotic-prone normal subjects. , 1993, British journal of psychology.

[123]  Thomas E. Hazy,et al.  Modelling Natural Action Selection: Toward an executive without a homunculus: computational models of the prefrontal cortex/basal ganglia system , 2011 .

[124]  T. Braver,et al.  Anterior Cingulate and the Monitoring of Response Conflict: Evidence from an fMRI Study of Overt Verb Generation , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[125]  L. Tremblay,et al.  Effects of dopamine agonists on the spontaneous activity of globus pallidus neurons in monkeys with MPTP-induced parkinsonism , 1991, Brain Research.

[126]  M. Horstink,et al.  Cognitive and motor shifting aptitude disorder in Parkinson's disease. , 1984, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[127]  C. Marsden,et al.  Cognitive function in Parkinson's disease: From description to theory , 1990, Trends in Neurosciences.

[128]  S. Kish,et al.  Uneven pattern of dopamine loss in the striatum of patients with idiopathic Parkinson's disease. Pathophysiologic and clinical implications. , 1988, The New England journal of medicine.

[129]  M. Freedman,et al.  A neuropsychological comparison of demented and nondemented patients with Parkinson's disease. , 1999, Applied neuropsychology.

[130]  A. Dickinson,et al.  Neuronal coding of prediction errors. , 2000, Annual review of neuroscience.

[131]  C. Marsden,et al.  l-Dopa withdrawal in Parkinson's disease selectively impairs cognitive performance in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction , 2005, Psychopharmacology.

[132]  M. Hoehn,et al.  Parkinsonism , 1967, Neurology.

[133]  Margaret H. Sharpe,et al.  Distractibility in Early Parkinson's Disease , 1990, Cortex.

[134]  H. Nelson A Modified Card Sorting Test Sensitive to Frontal Lobe Defects , 1976, Cortex.

[135]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Hold Your Horses: Impulsivity, Deep Brain Stimulation, and Medication in Parkinsonism , 2007, Science.

[136]  S Channon,et al.  Impairments in deductive reasoning and working memory in Parkinson's disease. , 1997, Behavioural neurology.

[137]  L. Squire,et al.  Parallel brain systems for learning with and without awareness. , 1994, Learning & memory.

[138]  T. Robbins,et al.  L-DOPA Disrupts Activity in the Nucleus Accumbens during Reversal Learning in Parkinson's Disease , 2007, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[139]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[140]  T. Braver,et al.  Functional specializations in lateral prefrontal cortex associated with the integration and segregation of information in working memory. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[141]  J. Saint-Cyr,et al.  Frontal lobe dysfunction in Parkinson's disease. The cortical focus of neostriatal outflow. , 1986, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[142]  A. Miyake,et al.  Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control , 1999 .

[143]  F. Gregory Ashby,et al.  THE ROLE OF THE BASAL GANGLIA IN CATEGORY LEARNING , 2006 .

[144]  R. Beninger,et al.  Impaired Incentive Learning in Treated Parkinson’s Disease , 1996, Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences / Journal Canadien des Sciences Neurologiques.

[145]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Error-Related Negativity Predicts Reinforcement Learning and Conflict Biases , 2005, Neuron.

[146]  M. Farah,et al.  Effects of bromocriptine on human subjects depend on working memory capacity , 1997, Neuroreport.

[147]  J. Stout,et al.  Enhanced negative priming in Parkinson's disease. , 2002, Neuropsychology.

[148]  M. Gluck,et al.  Human midbrain sensitivity to cognitive feedback and uncertainty during classification learning. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[149]  L. Defebvre,et al.  Impairment of the supervisory attentional system in early untreated patients with Parkinson’s disease , 1999, Journal of Neurology.

[150]  R. Lubow,et al.  The effects of target and distractor familiarity on visual search in de novo Parkinson's disease patients: latent inhibition and novel pop-out. , 1999, Neuropsychology.

[151]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.

[152]  J. Kruschke,et al.  Rule-based extrapolation in perceptual categorization , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[153]  J. Penney,et al.  The functional anatomy of basal ganglia disorders , 1989, Trends in Neurosciences.

[154]  A. M. Owen,et al.  Visuospatial memory deficits at different stages of Parkinson's disease , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[155]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Role of locus coeruleus in attention and behavioral flexibility , 1999, Biological Psychiatry.

[156]  P. T. Fox,et al.  Positron emission tomographic studies of the cortical anatomy of single-word processing , 1988, Nature.

[157]  Peter Dayan,et al.  A Neural Substrate of Prediction and Reward , 1997, Science.

[158]  B. Milner Effects of Different Brain Lesions on Card Sorting: The Role of the Frontal Lobes , 1963 .

[159]  T. Shallice,et al.  Qualitatively different memory impairments across frontal lobe subgroups , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[160]  J. Cohen,et al.  Context, cortex, and dopamine: a connectionist approach to behavior and biology in schizophrenia. , 1992, Psychological review.

[161]  T. Robbins,et al.  Contrasting mechanisms of impaired attentional set-shifting in patients with frontal lobe damage or Parkinson's disease. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[162]  M. Piccirilli,et al.  Frontal lobe dysfunction in Parkinson's disease: prognostic value for dementia? , 1989, European neurology.

[163]  F. Fornai,et al.  Noradrenaline in Parkinson's disease: from disease progression to current therapeutics. , 2007, Current medicinal chemistry.

[164]  T. Robbins,et al.  Cognitive Impairments in Early Parkinson's Disease Are Accompanied by Reductions in Activity in Frontostriatal Neural Circuitry , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[165]  R. Heaton Wisconsin Card Sorting Test manual , 1993 .

[166]  M. Delgado,et al.  Dorsal striatum responses to reward and punishment: Effects of valence and magnitude manipulations , 2003, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[167]  A. Tröster,et al.  Construct validity of the WCST in normal elderly and persons with Parkinson's disease. , 1995, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[168]  H. Mayberg,et al.  Depression and cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease. , 1989, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[169]  J. Kruschke,et al.  Rules and exemplars in category learning. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[170]  G. Mogenson,et al.  Limbic-motor integration , 1987 .

[171]  M Richards,et al.  Executive function in Parkinson's disease: set-shifting or set-maintenance? , 1993, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[172]  J. Hollerman,et al.  Dopamine neurons report an error in the temporal prediction of reward during learning , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[173]  G. Robertson,et al.  Evidence that the substantia nigra is a site of action for l-DOPA , 1988, Neuroscience Letters.

[174]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  A Biologically Based Computational Model of Working Memory , 1999 .

[175]  Tim Shallice,et al.  Specific impairments of rule induction in different frontal lobe subgroups , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[176]  T. Klingberg,et al.  Prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia control access to working memory , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[177]  J. Kulisevsky,et al.  Acute effects of levodopa on neuropsychological performance in stable and fluctuating Parkinson's disease patients at different levodopa plasma levels. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[178]  S. Foote,et al.  Extrathalamic modulation of cortical function. , 1987, Annual review of neuroscience.

[179]  S. Aalto,et al.  Striatal subregional 6‐[18F]fluoro‐L‐dopa uptake in early Parkinson's disease: A two‐year follow‐up study , 2006, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[180]  T. Mexia,et al.  Author ' s personal copy , 2009 .

[181]  J A Obeso,et al.  Pathophysiology of levodopa-induced dyskinesias in Parkinson's disease: problems with the current model. , 2000, Annals of neurology.

[182]  E. Sullivan,et al.  Cognitive impairment in early, untreated Parkinson's disease and its relationship to motor disability. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[183]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Reversal learning in Parkinson's disease depends on medication status and outcome valence , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[184]  Thomas E. Hazy,et al.  Towards an executive without a homunculus: computational models of the prefrontal cortex/basal ganglia system , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[185]  S. Lewis,et al.  The role of learned irrelevance in attentional set-shifting impairments in Parkinson's disease. , 2006, Neuropsychology.

[186]  R. Desimone,et al.  Internal globus pallidus discharge is nearly suppressed during levodopa‐induced dyskinesias , 1999, Annals of neurology.

[187]  John G. Taylor,et al.  Analysis of recurrent cortico-basal ganglia-thalamic loops for working memory , 2000, Biological Cybernetics.

[188]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Making Working Memory Work: A Computational Model of Learning in the Prefrontal Cortex and Basal Ganglia , 2006, Neural Computation.

[189]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  A mechanistic account of striatal dopamine function in human cognition: psychopharmacological studies with cabergoline and haloperidol. , 2006, Behavioral neuroscience.

[190]  W. T. Maddox,et al.  Striatal contributions to category learning: Quantitative modeling of simple linear and complex nonlinear rule learning in patients with Parkinson's disease , 2001, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[191]  M. M. Burns,et al.  Parkinsonism , 1975, Neurology.

[192]  Roshan Cools,et al.  Using executive heterogeneity to explore the nature of working memory deficits in Parkinson’s disease , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[193]  T. Robbins,et al.  Lesions of the dorsal noradrenergic bundle impair attentional set‐shifting in the rat , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[194]  Dean C. Delis,et al.  Visual selective attention deficits in patients with Parkinson's disease: A quantitative model-based approach. , 1996 .

[195]  A. C. Roberts,et al.  Impaired extra-dimensional shift performance in medicated and unmedicated Parkinson's disease: Evidence for a specific attentional dysfunction , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[196]  Robert K. Heaton,et al.  Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual – Revised and Expanded , 1993 .