The independent components of natural images are perceptually dependent

The independent components of natural images are a set of linear filters which are optimized for statistical independence. With such a set of filters images can be represented without loss of information. Intriguingly, the filter shapes are localized, oriented, and bandpass, resembling important properties of V1 simple cell receptive fields. Here we address the question of whether the independent components of natural images are also perceptually less dependent than other image components. We compared the pixel basis, the ICA basis and the discrete cosine basis by asking subjects to interactively predict missing pixels (for the pixel basis) or to predict the coefficients of ICA and DCT basis functions in patches of natural images. Like Kersten (1987)1 we find the pixel basis to be perceptually highly redundant but perhaps surprisingly, the ICA basis showed significantly higher perceptual dependencies than the DCT basis. This shows a dissociation between statistical and perceptual dependence measures.

[1]  K. Koffka Principles Of Gestalt Psychology , 1936 .

[2]  Claude E. Shannon,et al.  Prediction and Entropy of Printed English , 1951 .

[3]  S. W. Kuffler Discharge patterns and functional organization of mammalian retina. , 1953, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  F. Attneave Some informational aspects of visual perception. , 1954, Psychological review.

[5]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields of single neurones in the cat's striate cortex , 1959, The Journal of physiology.

[6]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex , 1962, The Journal of physiology.

[7]  C. Enroth-Cugell,et al.  The contrast sensitivity of retinal ganglion cells of the cat , 1966, The Journal of physiology.

[8]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex , 1968, The Journal of physiology.

[9]  J. Robson,et al.  Application of fourier analysis to the visibility of gratings , 1968, The Journal of physiology.

[10]  C Blakemore,et al.  On the existence of neurones in the human visual system selectively sensitive to the orientation and size of retinal images , 1969, The Journal of physiology.

[11]  Herbert A. Simon,et al.  The Sciences of the Artificial , 1970 .

[12]  D. Broadbent,et al.  Some experiments bearing on the hypothesis that the visual system analyses spatial patterns in independent bands of spatial frequency , 1975, Vision Research.

[13]  D J Field,et al.  Relations between the statistics of natural images and the response properties of cortical cells. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[14]  D Kersten,et al.  Predictability and redundancy of natural images. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[15]  A. Derrington,et al.  Some observations on the masking effects of two-dimensional stimuli , 1989, Vision Research.

[16]  E. Adelson,et al.  The Plenoptic Function and the Elements of Early Vision , 1991 .

[17]  J. V. van Hateren,et al.  Spatiotemporal contrast sensitivity of early vision , 1993, Vision Research.

[18]  J. H. Van Hateren,et al.  Spatiotemporal contrast sensitivity of early vision , 1993, Vision Research.

[19]  David J. Field,et al.  What Is the Goal of Sensory Coding? , 1994, Neural Computation.

[20]  David J. Field,et al.  Emergence of simple-cell receptive field properties by learning a sparse code for natural images , 1996, Nature.

[21]  David J. Field,et al.  Sparse coding with an overcomplete basis set: A strategy employed by V1? , 1997, Vision Research.

[22]  Eero P. Simoncelli Statistical models for images: compression, restoration and synthesis , 1997, Conference Record of the Thirty-First Asilomar Conference on Signals, Systems and Computers (Cat. No.97CB36136).

[23]  Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al.  The “independent components” of natural scenes are edge filters , 1997, Vision Research.

[24]  J. V. van Hateren,et al.  Independent component filters of natural images compared with simple cells in primary visual cortex , 1998, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[25]  J. H. Hateren,et al.  Independent component filters of natural images compared with simple cells in primary visual cortex , 1998 .

[26]  D. Ruderman,et al.  Independent component analysis of natural image sequences yields spatio-temporal filters similar to simple cells in primary visual cortex , 1998, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[27]  Aapo Hyvärinen,et al.  Emergence of Phase- and Shift-Invariant Features by Decomposition of Natural Images into Independent Feature Subspaces , 2000, Neural Computation.

[28]  Aapo Hyvärinen,et al.  A two-layer sparse coding model learns simple and complex cell receptive fields and topography from natural images , 2001, Vision Research.

[29]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  Natural image statistics and neural representation. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[30]  G. Henning,et al.  Contrast discrimination with pulse trains in pink noise. , 2002, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[31]  J. H. van Hateren,et al.  A theory of maximizing sensory information , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[32]  Steven W. Zucker,et al.  Complexity, Confusion, and Perceptual Grouping. Part I: The Curve-like Representation , 2004, International Journal of Computer Vision.

[33]  David J. Field,et al.  How Close Are We to Understanding V1? , 2005, Neural Computation.

[34]  M. Bethge Factorial coding of natural images: how effective are linear models in removing higher-order dependencies? , 2006, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[35]  G. Henning,et al.  Some observations on the pedestal effect. , 2007, Journal of vision.