Glass-pattern detection is tuned for stereo-depth
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Daniel C Kiper,et al. The detection of colored Glass patterns. , 2003, Journal of vision.
[2] P. O. Bishop,et al. Analysis of retinal correspondence by studying receptive fields of rinocular single units in cat striate cortex , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[3] E. Bizzi,et al. The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1996 .
[4] E. DeYoe,et al. Concurrent processing in the primate visual cortex. , 1995 .
[5] David J. Field,et al. Contour integration by the human visual system: Evidence for a local “association field” , 1993, Vision Research.
[6] Keiji Tanaka,et al. Neuronal selectivities to complex object features in the ventral visual pathway of the macaque cerebral cortex. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.
[7] D. Earle. Perception of Glass Pattern Structure with Stereopsis , 1985, Perception.
[8] N. Logothetis,et al. Role of the color-opponent and broad-band channels in vision , 1990, Visual Neuroscience.
[9] Chien-Chung Chen,et al. The human brain responses to Glass patterns: The effects of signal to noise ratio , 2004 .
[10] C. Wheatstone. XVIII. Contributions to the physiology of vision. —Part the first. On some remarkable, and hitherto unobserved, phenomena of binocular vision , 1962, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London.
[11] L. Glass. Moiré Effect from Random Dots , 1969, Nature.
[12] J. Ross. The perceived direction and speed of global motion in Glass pattern sequences , 2004, Vision Research.
[13] David R. Badcock,et al. Interactions between luminance and contrast signals in global form detection , 2005, Vision Research.
[14] I. Fujita,et al. Disparity selectivity of neurons in monkey inferior temporal cortex. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.
[15] E. DeYoe,et al. Segregation of efferent connections and receptive field properties in visual area V2 of the macaque , 1985, Nature.
[16] Frank Bremmer,et al. Neural correlates of implied motion , 2003, Nature.
[17] S C Dakin,et al. Glass Patterns: Some Contrast Effects Re-Evaluated , 1997, Perception.
[18] P. Lennie. Single Units and Visual Cortical Organization , 1998, Perception.
[19] J. Gallant,et al. A Human Extrastriate Area Functionally Homologous to Macaque V4 , 2000, Neuron.
[20] D R Badcock,et al. Global form perception: interactions between luminance and texture information. , 1999, Australian and New Zealand journal of ophthalmology.
[21] L. Glass,et al. Pattern Recognition in Humans: Correlations Which Cannot be Perceived , 1976, Perception.
[22] R. Pérez,et al. Perception of Random Dot Interference Patterns , 1973, Nature.
[23] Vision Research , 1961, Nature.
[24] D. Hubel,et al. Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex , 1968, The Journal of physiology.
[25] ROBERT F. HESS,et al. Integrating Contours Within and Through Depth , 1997, Vision Research.
[26] H. Wilson,et al. Concentric orientation summation in human form vision , 1997, Vision Research.
[27] B. Julesz. Foundations of Cyclopean Perception , 1971 .
[28] C. Connor,et al. Three-dimensional orientation tuning in macaque area V4 , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.
[29] B Julesz,et al. Psychophysical evidence for global feature processing in visual texture discrimination. , 1979, Journal of the Optical Society of America.
[30] S. Dakin,et al. Local and global visual grouping: tuning for spatial frequency and contrast. , 2001, Journal of vision.
[31] J. A Wilson,et al. Glass pattern studies of local and global processing of contrast variations , 2004, Vision Research.
[32] Ichiro Fujita,et al. Disparity-selective neurons in area V4 of macaque monkeys. , 2002 .
[33] D. V. van Essen,et al. Selectivity for polar, hyperbolic, and Cartesian gratings in macaque visual cortex. , 1993, Science.
[34] G. Poggio,et al. Binocular interaction and depth sensitivity in striate and prestriate cortex of behaving rhesus monkey. , 1977, Journal of neurophysiology.
[35] Anthony J. Movshon,et al. Signals in Macaque Striate Cortical Neurons that Support the Perception of Glass Patterns , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[36] Eugene Switkes. Integration of differing chromaticities in early and midlevel spatial vision , 2002 .
[37] L. Chalupa,et al. The visual neurosciences , 2004 .
[38] D. Hubel,et al. Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex , 1962, The Journal of physiology.
[39] H. Wilson,et al. Detection of global structure in Glass patterns: implications for form vision , 1998, Vision Research.
[40] David J. Field,et al. Contour integration across depth , 1995, Vision Research.
[41] D. C. Essen,et al. Neural responses to polar, hyperbolic, and Cartesian gratings in area V4 of the macaque monkey. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.
[42] C. Blakemore,et al. The neural mechanism of binocular depth discrimination , 1967, The Journal of physiology.
[43] Charles Wheatstone. On some remarkable and hitherto unobserved phenomena of binocular vision. , 1962 .
[44] David R. Badcock,et al. Coherent global motion in the absence of coherent velocity signals , 2000, Current Biology.
[45] Ravi S. Menon,et al. An fMRI study of the selective activation of human extrastriate form vision areas by radial and concentric gratings , 2000, Current Biology.
[46] Frederick A A Kingdom,et al. Color brings relief to human vision , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[47] H. Barlow,et al. Limit to the detection of Glass patterns in the presence of noise. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[48] David R. Badcock,et al. Motion distorts perceived depth , 2003, Vision Research.