Glass-pattern detection is tuned for stereo-depth

We investigated the role of disparity information in the detection of global form. Glass patterns, which allow insight into processing at both local and global stages of form analysis, were used as stimuli. We determined how detection of concentric Glass patterns is affected by a disparity difference introduced between partner dots forming local dipoles (Experiment 1), and how detection is affected by the addition of randomly oriented dot-pairs (noise dots) at crossed and uncrossed disparities (Experiment 2). The first experiment showed that detection thresholds increased when partner dots were separated in depth at disparities greater than approximately 17 min arc; the second experiment showed that noise dots disrupted the detection of form if they were presented at disparities of between approximately +/-20 min arc from the Glass pattern's presentation depth plane. Our findings suggest that disparity information plays a role in the recovery of the image structure and, importantly, local and global form mechanisms were found to be selective for a small range of stereo-depths. We discuss the findings of our study in the light of current evidence indicating that a common neural substrate is responsible for the analysis of form and binocular disparity.

[1]  Daniel C Kiper,et al.  The detection of colored Glass patterns. , 2003, Journal of vision.

[2]  P. O. Bishop,et al.  Analysis of retinal correspondence by studying receptive fields of rinocular single units in cat striate cortex , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  E. Bizzi,et al.  The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1996 .

[4]  E. DeYoe,et al.  Concurrent processing in the primate visual cortex. , 1995 .

[5]  David J. Field,et al.  Contour integration by the human visual system: Evidence for a local “association field” , 1993, Vision Research.

[6]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Neuronal selectivities to complex object features in the ventral visual pathway of the macaque cerebral cortex. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  D. Earle Perception of Glass Pattern Structure with Stereopsis , 1985, Perception.

[8]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Role of the color-opponent and broad-band channels in vision , 1990, Visual Neuroscience.

[9]  Chien-Chung Chen,et al.  The human brain responses to Glass patterns: The effects of signal to noise ratio , 2004 .

[10]  C. Wheatstone XVIII. Contributions to the physiology of vision. —Part the first. On some remarkable, and hitherto unobserved, phenomena of binocular vision , 1962, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London.

[11]  L. Glass Moiré Effect from Random Dots , 1969, Nature.

[12]  J. Ross The perceived direction and speed of global motion in Glass pattern sequences , 2004, Vision Research.

[13]  David R. Badcock,et al.  Interactions between luminance and contrast signals in global form detection , 2005, Vision Research.

[14]  I. Fujita,et al.  Disparity selectivity of neurons in monkey inferior temporal cortex. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  E. DeYoe,et al.  Segregation of efferent connections and receptive field properties in visual area V2 of the macaque , 1985, Nature.

[16]  Frank Bremmer,et al.  Neural correlates of implied motion , 2003, Nature.

[17]  S C Dakin,et al.  Glass Patterns: Some Contrast Effects Re-Evaluated , 1997, Perception.

[18]  P. Lennie Single Units and Visual Cortical Organization , 1998, Perception.

[19]  J. Gallant,et al.  A Human Extrastriate Area Functionally Homologous to Macaque V4 , 2000, Neuron.

[20]  D R Badcock,et al.  Global form perception: interactions between luminance and texture information. , 1999, Australian and New Zealand journal of ophthalmology.

[21]  L. Glass,et al.  Pattern Recognition in Humans: Correlations Which Cannot be Perceived , 1976, Perception.

[22]  R. Pérez,et al.  Perception of Random Dot Interference Patterns , 1973, Nature.

[23]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[24]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex , 1968, The Journal of physiology.

[25]  ROBERT F. HESS,et al.  Integrating Contours Within and Through Depth , 1997, Vision Research.

[26]  H. Wilson,et al.  Concentric orientation summation in human form vision , 1997, Vision Research.

[27]  B. Julesz Foundations of Cyclopean Perception , 1971 .

[28]  C. Connor,et al.  Three-dimensional orientation tuning in macaque area V4 , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[29]  B Julesz,et al.  Psychophysical evidence for global feature processing in visual texture discrimination. , 1979, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[30]  S. Dakin,et al.  Local and global visual grouping: tuning for spatial frequency and contrast. , 2001, Journal of vision.

[31]  J. A Wilson,et al.  Glass pattern studies of local and global processing of contrast variations , 2004, Vision Research.

[32]  Ichiro Fujita,et al.  Disparity-selective neurons in area V4 of macaque monkeys. , 2002 .

[33]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  Selectivity for polar, hyperbolic, and Cartesian gratings in macaque visual cortex. , 1993, Science.

[34]  G. Poggio,et al.  Binocular interaction and depth sensitivity in striate and prestriate cortex of behaving rhesus monkey. , 1977, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  Anthony J. Movshon,et al.  Signals in Macaque Striate Cortical Neurons that Support the Perception of Glass Patterns , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  Eugene Switkes Integration of differing chromaticities in early and midlevel spatial vision , 2002 .

[37]  L. Chalupa,et al.  The visual neurosciences , 2004 .

[38]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex , 1962, The Journal of physiology.

[39]  H. Wilson,et al.  Detection of global structure in Glass patterns: implications for form vision , 1998, Vision Research.

[40]  David J. Field,et al.  Contour integration across depth , 1995, Vision Research.

[41]  D. C. Essen,et al.  Neural responses to polar, hyperbolic, and Cartesian gratings in area V4 of the macaque monkey. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  C. Blakemore,et al.  The neural mechanism of binocular depth discrimination , 1967, The Journal of physiology.

[43]  Charles Wheatstone On some remarkable and hitherto unobserved phenomena of binocular vision. , 1962 .

[44]  David R. Badcock,et al.  Coherent global motion in the absence of coherent velocity signals , 2000, Current Biology.

[45]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  An fMRI study of the selective activation of human extrastriate form vision areas by radial and concentric gratings , 2000, Current Biology.

[46]  Frederick A A Kingdom,et al.  Color brings relief to human vision , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[47]  H. Barlow,et al.  Limit to the detection of Glass patterns in the presence of noise. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[48]  David R. Badcock,et al.  Motion distorts perceived depth , 2003, Vision Research.