The neural correlates of theory of mind and their role during empathy and the game of chess: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study

Chess involves the capacity to reason iteratively about potential intentional choices of an opponent and therefore involves high levels of explicit theory of mind [ToM] (i.e. ability to infer mental states of others) alongside clear, strategic rule-based decision-making. Functional magnetic resonance imaging was used on 12 healthy male novice chess players to identify cortical regions associated with chess, ToM and empathizing. The blood-oxygenation-level-dependent (BOLD) response for chess and empathizing tasks was extracted from each ToM region. Results showed neural overlap between ToM, chess and empathizing tasks in right-hemisphere temporo-parietal junction (TPJ) [BA40], left-hemisphere superior temporal gyrus [BA22] and posterior cingulate gyrus [BA23/31]. TPJ is suggested to underlie the capacity to reason iteratively about another's internal state in a range of tasks. Areas activated by ToM and empathy included right-hemisphere orbitofrontal cortex and bilateral middle temporal gyrus: areas that become active when there is need to inhibit one's own experience when considering the internal state of another and for visual evaluation of action rationality. Results support previous findings, that ToM recruits a neural network with each region sub-serving a supporting role depending on the nature of the task itself. In contrast, a network of cortical regions primarily located within right- and left-hemisphere medial-frontal and parietal cortex, outside the internal representational network, was selectively recruited during the chess task. We hypothesize that in our cohort of novice chess players the strategy was to employ an iterative thinking pattern which in part involved mentalizing processes and recruited core ToM-related regions.

[1]  Sheng He,et al.  A functional MRI study of high-level cognition. I. The game of chess. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[2]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  Orbital prefrontal cortex volume predicts social network size: an imaging study of individual differences in humans , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[3]  P. Buckley,et al.  The Rupture and Repair of Cooperation in Borderline Personality Disorder , 2009 .

[4]  Graham J. Kemp,et al.  Association between language and spatial laterality and cognitive ability: An fMRI study , 2012, NeuroImage.

[5]  Michael B. Miller,et al.  Neural Correlates of Detecting Pretense: Automatic Engagement of the Intentional Stance under Covert Conditions , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[6]  Bruno G. Bara,et al.  Intention Processing in Communication: A Common Brain Network for Language and Gestures , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[7]  K. Zilles,et al.  The Neural Basis of Vertical and Horizontal Line Bisection Judgments: An fMRI Study of Normal Volunteers , 2001, NeuroImage.

[8]  Matthew F S Rushworth,et al.  The Computation of Social Behavior , 2009, Science.

[9]  Penelope A. Lewis,et al.  Ventromedial prefrontal volume predicts understanding of others and social network size , 2011, NeuroImage.

[10]  Dana Samson,et al.  Seeing it my way: a case of a selective deficit in inhibiting self-perspective. , 2005, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  Aaron C. Koralek,et al.  Two Takes on the Social Brain: A Comparison of Theory of Mind Tasks , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[12]  S. Carey,et al.  Understanding other minds: linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging. , 2004, Annual review of psychology.

[13]  Michael J. Brammer,et al.  The role of ‘shared representations’ in social perception and empathy: An fMRI study , 2006, NeuroImage.

[14]  J. Perner,et al.  Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews Fractionating Theory of Mind: a Meta-analysis of Functional Brain Imaging Studies , 2022 .

[15]  E. Bora,et al.  Evidence for theory of mind deficits in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder , 2005, Acta psychiatrica Scandinavica.

[16]  Michael Erb,et al.  Mechanisms and neural basis of object and pattern recognition: a study with chess experts. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[17]  Baris Korkmaz,et al.  Theory of Mind and Neurodevelopmental Disorders of Childhood , 2011, Pediatric Research.

[18]  Fernand Gobet,et al.  A pattern-recognition theory of search in expert problem solving , 1997 .

[19]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  The Empathy Quotient: An Investigation of Adults with Asperger Syndrome or High Functioning Autism, and Normal Sex Differences , 2004, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[20]  C. Frith,et al.  Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[21]  A. Bailey,et al.  Are there theory of mind regions in the brain? A review of the neuroimaging literature , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[22]  David F. Sally,et al.  The development of interpersonal strategy: Autism, theory-of-mind, cooperation and fairness , 2006 .

[23]  K. Zilles,et al.  Mind Reading: Neural Mechanisms of Theory of Mind and Self-Perspective , 2001, NeuroImage.

[24]  I. Wilkinson,et al.  Investigating the functional anatomy of empathy and forgiveness , 2001, Neuroreport.

[25]  ChrisD . Frith The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia , 1992 .

[26]  V. Gallese The Roots of Empathy: The Shared Manifold Hypothesis and the Neural Basis of Intersubjectivity , 2003, Psychopathology.

[27]  Fernand Gobet,et al.  Chess players' thinking revisited , 1998 .

[28]  Joseph A Maldjian,et al.  Precentral gyrus discrepancy in electronic versions of the Talairach atlas , 2004, NeuroImage.

[29]  G. Orban,et al.  Human Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Reveals Separation and Integration of Shape and Motion Cues in Biological Motion Processing , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  James Stiller,et al.  Perspective-taking and memory capacity predict social network size , 2007, Soc. Networks.

[31]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  Social cognitive neuroscience: a review of core processes. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.

[32]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Analysis of functional MRI time‐series , 1994, Human Brain Mapping.

[33]  K. Zilles,et al.  Line bisection judgments implicate right parietal cortex and cerebellum as assessed by fMRI , 2000, Neurology.

[34]  Neil Charness,et al.  The role of deliberate practice in chess expertise , 2005 .

[35]  Cindy M. Bukach,et al.  Beyond faces and modularity: the power of an expertise framework , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[36]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Distinct neural systems subserve person and object knowledge , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[37]  J. Decety,et al.  A PET Investigation of the Attribution of Intentions with a Nonverbal Task , 2000, NeuroImage.

[38]  M. Tarr,et al.  Are chess experts any different from face, bird, or Greeble experts? , 2004 .

[39]  Colin F. Camerer,et al.  Behavioral Game Theory and the Neural Basis of Strategic Choice , 2009 .

[40]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Spatial registration and normalization of images , 1995 .

[41]  Christopher D. Frith,et al.  Imaging the Intentional Stance in a Competitive Game , 2002, NeuroImage.

[42]  Neil Roberts,et al.  Different association between intentionality competence and prefrontal volume in left- and right-handers , 2014, Cortex.

[43]  Rebecca Elliott,et al.  Neuronal correlates of theory of mind and empathy: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study in a nonverbal task , 2006, NeuroImage.

[44]  W. Grodd,et al.  Many Faces of Expertise: Fusiform Face Area in Chess Experts and Novices , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  Michael Erb,et al.  It Takes Two–Skilled Recognition of Objects Engages Lateral Areas in Both Hemispheres , 2011, PloS one.

[46]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  Orbital prefrontal cortex volume correlates with social cognitive competence , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[47]  R. Nathan Spreng,et al.  The Common Neural Basis of Autobiographical Memory, Prospection, Navigation, Theory of Mind, and the Default Mode: A Quantitative Meta-analysis , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[48]  H. Simon,et al.  Perception in chess , 1973 .

[49]  M. Tateno,et al.  The Study of Cognitive Characteristics in Asperger’s Disorder by Using a Modified Prisoner’s Dilemma Game with a Variable Payoff Matrix , 2012, PloS one.

[50]  I. Daum,et al.  Social cognition in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[51]  Sheng He,et al.  A functional MRI study of high-level cognition , 2003 .

[52]  C. Frith,et al.  Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[53]  A. Cavanna,et al.  Social reasoning in Tourette syndrome , 2011, Cognitive neuropsychiatry.

[54]  T. Singer,et al.  The neural basis of empathy. , 2012, Annual review of neuroscience.

[55]  Michael Erb,et al.  Expertise modulates the neural basis of context dependent recognition of objects and their relations , 2012, Human brain mapping.

[56]  R. Saxe,et al.  Making sense of another mind: The role of the right temporo-parietal junction , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[57]  P. Grant,et al.  Dopaminergic foundations of schizotypy as measured by the German version of the Oxford-Liverpool Inventory of Feelings and Experiences (O-LIFE)—a suitable endophenotype of schizophrenia , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[58]  I. Wilkinson,et al.  Behavioural and functional anatomical correlates of deception in humans , 2001, Neuroreport.

[59]  M. Sigman,et al.  Conversational Abilities Among Children with Autism and Children with Developmental Delays , 1998 .

[60]  R Saxe,et al.  People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.

[61]  C. Frith,et al.  The Neural Basis of Mentalizing , 2006, Neuron.

[62]  U. Frith,et al.  The neuropsychology of autism. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[63]  C. Frith,et al.  Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[64]  A. Georgopoulos,et al.  Time‐resolved fMRI of mental rotation , 1997, Neuroreport.

[65]  T. Allison,et al.  Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[66]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Empathy for Pain Involves the Affective but not Sensory Components of Pain , 2004, Science.

[67]  C. Frith,et al.  Interacting minds--a biological basis. , 1999, Science.

[68]  R. Marshall,et al.  Changes in Regional Cerebral Blood Flow Related to Line Bisection Discrimination and Visual Attention Using HMPAO-SPECT , 1997, NeuroImage.

[69]  G. Orban,et al.  Neural mechanisms of understanding rational actions: middle temporal gyrus activation by contextual violation. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[70]  H. Karnath,et al.  The temporo-parietal junction contributes to global gestalt perception—evidence from studies in chess experts , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[71]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  The "Reading the Mind in the Eyes" Test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. , 2001, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[72]  Fernand Gobet,et al.  Perception and Memory in Chess , 1996, J. Int. Comput. Games Assoc..

[73]  Jason P. Mitchell Inferences about mental states , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[74]  Daniel C. Krawczyk,et al.  The neural organization of perception in chess experts , 2011, Neuroscience Letters.

[75]  Fernand Gobet,et al.  Structure and Stimulus Familiarity: A Study of Memory in Chess-Players with Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2005, The Spanish Journal of Psychology.

[76]  Daniel Houser,et al.  A functional imaging study of cooperation in two-person reciprocal exchange , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[77]  A. Elo The rating of chessplayers, past and present , 1978 .

[78]  A. D. D. Groot,et al.  Perception and memory in chess: Studies in the heuristics of the prodessional eye , 1996 .

[79]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Analysis of fMRI Time-Series Revisited , 1995, NeuroImage.

[80]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Neuroeconomics: How Neuroscience Can Inform Economics , 2005 .

[81]  S. Shamay-Tsoory,et al.  Neuroanatomical and neurochemical bases of theory of mind , 2011, Neuropsychologia.