Implicit adaptation compensates for erratic explicit strategy in human motor learning

Sports are replete with strategies, yet coaching lore often emphasizes ‘quieting the mind’, ‘trusting the body’ and ‘avoiding overthinking’ in referring to the importance of relying less on high-level explicit strategies in favor of low-level implicit motor learning. We investigated the interactions between explicit strategy and implicit motor adaptation by designing a sensorimotor learning paradigm that drives adaptive changes in some dimensions but not others. We find that strategy and implicit adaptation synergize in driven dimensions, but effectively cancel each other in undriven dimensions. Independent analyses—based on time lags, the correlational structure in the data and computational modeling—demonstrate that this cancellation occurs because implicit adaptation effectively compensates for noise in explicit strategy rather than the converse, acting to clean up the motor noise resulting from low-fidelity explicit strategy during motor learning. These results provide new insight into why implicit learning increasingly takes over from explicit strategy as skill learning proceeds. Implicit learning increases the fidelity of performance during motor learning by acting to adaptively clean up the noise resulting from a low-fidelity explicit strategy.

[1]  Krista M Bond,et al.  Flexible explicit but rigid implicit learning in a visuomotor adaptation task. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[3]  Howard G. Wu,et al.  The Generalization of Visuomotor Learning to Untrained Movements and Movement Sequences Based on Movement Vector and Goal Location Remapping , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  R. Gray Attending to the execution of a complex sensorimotor skill: expertise differences, choking, and slumps. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[5]  Joaquin A. Anguera,et al.  A spatial explicit strategy reduces error but interferes with sensorimotor adaptation. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Error correction, sensory prediction, and adaptation in motor control. , 2010, Annual review of neuroscience.

[7]  Gary C. Sing,et al.  Primitives for Motor Adaptation Reflect Correlated Neural Tuning to Position and Velocity , 2009, Neuron.

[8]  R. Ivry,et al.  Trial-by-trial analysis of intermanual transfer during visuomotor adaptation. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  C. Stern,et al.  An fMRI Study of the Role of the Medial Temporal Lobe in Implicit and Explicit Sequence Learning , 2003, Neuron.

[10]  J. Krakauer,et al.  An Implicit Plan Overrides an Explicit Strategy during Visuomotor Adaptation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  M. Packard,et al.  Differential effects of fornix and caudate nucleus lesions on two radial maze tasks: evidence for multiple memory systems , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[12]  Wilsaan M. Joiner,et al.  Dissociating effects of error size, training duration, and amount of adaptation on the ability to retain motor memories. , 2019, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  Yves Rosseel,et al.  lavaan: An R Package for Structural Equation Modeling , 2012 .

[14]  N. Ellis Rules and instances in foreign language learning: Interactions of explicit and implicit knowledge , 1993 .

[15]  Bernard Devauchelle,et al.  Principle of structural equation modeling for exploring functional interactivity within a putative network of interconnected brain areas. , 2009, Magnetic resonance imaging.

[16]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Explicit and Implicit Contributions to Learning in a Sensorimotor Adaptation Task , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Interacting Adaptive Processes with Different Timescales Underlie Short-Term Motor Learning , 2006, PLoS biology.

[18]  Andreas Ritter,et al.  Structural Equations With Latent Variables , 2016 .

[19]  Carolee J Winstein,et al.  Providing explicit information disrupts implicit motor learning after basal ganglia stroke. , 2004, Learning & memory.

[20]  L. Gugerty Situation Awareness During Driving: Explicit and Implicit Knowledge in Dynamic Spatial Memory , 1997 .

[21]  Mark J Wagner,et al.  Shared Internal Models for Feedforward and Feedback Control , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  W. T. Thach,et al.  Throwing while looking through prisms. I. Focal olivocerebellar lesions impair adaptation. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[23]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  The acquisition of skilled motor performance: fast and slow experience-driven changes in primary motor cortex. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[24]  David M. Huberdeau,et al.  The Influence of Movement Preparation Time on the Expression of Visuomotor Learning and Savings , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  C Ghez,et al.  Learning of Visuomotor Transformations for Vectorial Planning of Reaching Trajectories , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  Michael S. Landy,et al.  Measuring adaptation with a sinusoidal perturbation function , 2012, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[27]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Intact ability to learn internal models of arm dynamics in Huntington's disease but not cerebellar degeneration. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  Kelly J. Ashford,et al.  Attentional Focus, Dispositional Reinvestment, and Skilled Motor Performance under Pressure , 2006 .

[29]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Making smooth moves , 2022 .

[30]  R. Ivry,et al.  An Explicit Strategy Prevails When the Cerebellum Fails to Compute Movement Errors , 2010, The Cerebellum.

[31]  G. Stelmach,et al.  Adaptation to gradual as compared with sudden visuo-motor distortions , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  C. Stein,et al.  Structural equation modeling. , 2012, Methods in molecular biology.

[33]  John R. Anderson Acquisition of cognitive skill. , 1982 .

[34]  Rex B. Kline,et al.  Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling , 1998 .

[35]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Competition among multiple memory systems: converging evidence from animal and human brain studies , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  Richard B. Ivry,et al.  Flexible Cognitive Strategies during Motor Learning , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[37]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Human sensorimotor learning: adaptation, skill, and beyond , 2011, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[38]  M. Gluck,et al.  Interactive memory systems in the human brain , 2001, Nature.

[39]  L. Jakobson,et al.  Trajectories of reaches to prismatically-displaced targets: evidence for “automatic” visuomotor recalibration , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[40]  John W Krakauer,et al.  Motor learning and consolidation: the case of visuomotor rotation. , 2009, Advances in experimental medicine and biology.

[41]  Richard B. Ivry,et al.  Double dissociation of single-interval and rhythmic temporal prediction in cerebellar degeneration and Parkinson’s disease , 2018, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[42]  Kelvin E. Jones,et al.  Sources of signal-dependent noise during isometric force production. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  Maurice A. Smith,et al.  Motor Memory Is Encoded as a Gain-Field Combination of Intrinsic and Extrinsic Action Representations , 2012, Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  R. Held,et al.  PLASTICITY IN HUMAN SENSORIMOTOR CONTROL. , 1963, Science.

[45]  Allison M. Okamura,et al.  Cerebellar damage reduces the stability of motor memories , 2014 .

[46]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  Expertise, attention, and memory in sensorimotor skill execution: Impact of novel task constraints on dual-task performance and episodic memory , 2002, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[47]  A G Barto,et al.  Prediction of complex two-dimensional trajectories by a cerebellar model of smooth pursuit eye movement. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  Wilsaan M. Joiner,et al.  Long-term retention explained by a model of short-term learning in the adaptive control of reaching. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[49]  Michael C. Anderson,et al.  Overthinking skilled motor performance: Or why those who teach can’t do , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[50]  R. Ivry,et al.  Cerebellar involvement in anticipating the consequences of self-produced actions during bimanual movements. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.