Linear and non-linear mechanisms in the perception of stereoscopic slant and transparency.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Keith Langley,et al. A MODEL FOR COHERENT AND MULTIPLICATIVELY TRANSPARENT PLAIDS , 1995 .
[2] D. P. Andrews,et al. APE: Adaptive probit estimation of psychometric functions , 1981 .
[3] C. Tyler,et al. Depth from spatial frequency difference: An old kind of stereopsis? , 1979, Vision Research.
[4] Brian J. Rogers,et al. The Perception and Representation of Depth and Slant in Stereoscopic Surfaces , 1992 .
[5] D. Weinshall. Seeing “ghost” planes in stereo vision , 1991, Vision Research.
[6] B Gillam,et al. Evidence for disparity change as the primary stimulus for stereoscopic processing , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.
[7] I. Howard,et al. Relative shear disparities and the perception of surface inclination , 1994, Vision Research.
[8] J. Pokorny. Foundations of Cyclopean Perception , 1972 .
[9] Christopher W. Tyler,et al. Analysis of stereothresholds for stimuli below 2.5 c/deg , 1994, Vision Research.
[10] M. Georgeson,et al. Does early non-linearity account for second-order motion? , 1999, Vision Research.
[11] S. F. Bowne. Contrast discrimination cannot explain spatial frequency, orientation or temporal frequency discrimination , 1990, Vision Research.
[12] C. Sherrington. Integrative Action of the Nervous System , 1907 .
[13] P. Cavanagh,et al. Subjective contours capture stereopsis , 1985, Nature.
[14] C Blakemore,et al. On the existence of neurones in the human visual system selectively sensitive to the orientation and size of retinal images , 1969, The Journal of physiology.
[15] Randolph Blake,et al. What causes stereoscopic tilt from spatial frequency disparity , 1987, Vision Research.
[16] J. E. W. Mayhew,et al. A computational model of binocular depth perception , 1982, Nature.
[17] G. F. Cooper,et al. The angular selectivity of visual cortical cells to moving gratings , 1968, The Journal of physiology.
[18] David J. Fleet,et al. Linear filtering precedes nonlinear processing in early vision , 1996, Current Biology.
[19] R. L. de Valois,et al. Psychophysical studies of monkey vision. 3. Spatial luminance contrast sensitivity tests of macaque and human observers. , 1974, Vision research.
[20] G. F. Cooper,et al. The spatial selectivity of the visual cells of the cat , 1969, The Journal of physiology.
[21] Barbara Gillam,et al. Perspective, Orientation Disparity, and Anisotropy in Stereoscopic Slant Perception , 1992, Perception.
[22] J. Robson,et al. Application of fourier analysis to the visibility of gratings , 1968, The Journal of physiology.
[23] Charles Wheatstone. On some remarkable and hitherto unobserved phenomena of binocular vision. , 1962 .
[24] Vincent P. Ferrera,et al. Direction specific masking and the analysis of motion in two dimensions , 1987, Vision Research.
[25] F. Heitger,et al. The functional role of contrast adaptation , 1988, Vision Research.
[26] J. P. Jones,et al. The two-dimensional spatial structure of simple receptive fields in cat striate cortex. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.
[27] J. Daugman. Uncertainty relation for resolution in space, spatial frequency, and orientation optimized by two-dimensional visual cortical filters. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[28] RussLL L. Ds Vnlos,et al. SPATIAL FREQUENCY SELECTIVITY OF CELLS IN MACAQUE VISUAL CORTEX , 2022 .
[29] B. Julesz. Binocular depth perception of computer-generated patterns , 1960 .
[30] G. Orban,et al. Human orientation discrimination tested with long stimuli , 1984, Vision Research.
[31] J P Thomas,et al. Underlying psychometric function for detecting gratings and identifying spatial frequency. , 1983, Journal of the Optical Society of America.
[32] G. J. Burton,et al. Evidence for non-linear response processes in the human visual system from measurements on the thresholds of spatial beat frequencies. , 1973, Vision research.
[33] M F Bradshaw,et al. Disparity Scaling and the Perception of Frontoparallel Surfaces , 1995, Perception.
[34] H. C. Longuet-Higgins,et al. The interpretation of a moving retinal image , 1980, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.
[35] Vincent P. Ferrera,et al. Perceived speed of moving two-dimensional patterns , 1991, Vision Research.
[36] R. Blake,et al. Spatial frequency tuning of human stereopsis , 1991, Vision Research.
[37] J I Nelson,et al. Globality and stereoscopic fusion in binocular vision. , 1975, Journal of theoretical biology.
[38] C. William Tyler,et al. Characteristics of stereomovement suppression , 1975 .
[39] H. C. Longuet-Higgins. The Role of the Vertical Dimension in Stereoscopic Vision , 1982, Perception.
[40] B Gillam,et al. Orientation Disparity, Deformation, and Stereoscopic Slant Perception , 1991, Perception.
[41] Richard P. Wildes,et al. Direct Recovery of Three-Dimensional Scene Geometry From Binocular Stereo Disparity , 1991, IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell..
[42] Laurie M. Wilcox,et al. Is the site of non-linear filtering in stereopsis before or after binocular combination? , 1996, Vision Research.
[43] B. Rogers,et al. Disparity curvature and the perception of three-dimensional surfaces , 1989, Nature.
[44] P. Wenderoth. A Visual Spatial Aftereffect of Surface Slant , 1970 .
[45] H. Wilson,et al. A psychophysically motivated model for two-dimensional motion perception , 1992, Visual Neuroscience.
[46] D. W. Heeley,et al. Spatial frequency discrimination at different orientations , 1989, Vision Research.
[47] K. Langley,et al. Vertical and Horizontal Disparities from Phase , 1990, ECCV.
[48] A. Parker,et al. An orientation-tuned component in the contrast masking of stereopsis , 1993, Vision Research.
[49] L. Palmer,et al. Receptive-field structure in cat striate cortex. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.
[50] Christopher W. Tyler,et al. A stereoscopic view of visual processing streams , 1990, Vision Research.
[51] D. Broadbent,et al. Some experiments bearing on the hypothesis that the visual system analyses spatial patterns in independent bands of spatial frequency , 1975, Vision Research.
[52] Jitendra Malik,et al. Determining Three-Dimensional Shape from Orientation and Spatial Frequency Disparities , 1991, ECCV.
[53] B Gillam,et al. Postfusional latency in stereoscopic slant perception and the primitives of stereopsis. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[54] R. A. Smith,et al. Disparity processing of spatial frequencies in man , 1972, The Journal of physiology.
[55] R D Freeman,et al. Neuronal Mechanisms Underlying Stereopsis: How Do Simple Cells in the Visual Cortex Encode Binocular Disparity? , 1995, Perception.
[56] C. F. Stromeyer,et al. Low spatial-frequency channels in human vision: Adaptation and masking , 1982, Vision Research.
[57] B. Gillam,et al. Perception of slant when perspective and stereopsis conflict: experiments with aniseikonic lenses. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.
[58] I. Ohzawa,et al. Stereoscopic depth discrimination in the visual cortex: neurons ideally suited as disparity detectors. , 1990, Science.
[59] P. Wenderoth,et al. The effect of interactions between one-dimensional component gratings on two-dimensional motion perception , 1993, Vision Research.
[60] W E Grimson,et al. A computer implementation of a theory of human stereo vision. , 1981, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[61] A. Derrington,et al. Separate detectors for simple and complex grating patterns? , 1985, Vision Research.
[62] Daniel A. Pollen,et al. Visual cortical neurons as localized spatial frequency filters , 1983, IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics.
[63] D Marr,et al. A computational theory of human stereo vision. , 1979, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[64] Masanobu Yamamoto,et al. A General Aperture Problem for Direct Estimation of 3-D Motion Parameters , 1989, IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell..
[65] B Gillam,et al. A Proximity-Contingent Stereoscopic Depth Aftereffect: Evidence for Adaptation to Disparity Gradients , 1993, Perception.
[66] Y. J. Tejwani,et al. Robot vision , 1989, IEEE International Symposium on Circuits and Systems,.
[67] RANDOLPH BLAKE,et al. Stereopsis from Interocular Spatial Frequency Differences is Not Robust , 1996, Vision Research.
[68] M von Grünau,et al. The effect of disparity on motion coherence. , 1993, Spatial vision.
[69] Laurie M. Wilcox,et al. Linear and non-linear filtering in stereopsis , 1994, Vision Research.
[70] D. W. Heeley,et al. Meridional anisotropies of orientation discrimination for sine wave gratings , 1988, Vision Research.
[71] C. Blakemore,et al. A new kind of stereoscopic vision. , 1970, Vision research.
[72] C. Blakemore,et al. The orientation specificity of two visual after‐effects , 1971, The Journal of physiology.
[73] D. W. Heeley,et al. Evidence for separate, task dependent noise processes in orientation and size perception , 1994, Vision Research.
[74] C. Schor,et al. Disparity range for local stereopsis as a function of luminance spatial frequency , 1983, Vision Research.
[75] David C. Burr,et al. Orientation discrimination depends on spatial frequency , 1991, Vision Research.
[76] D. G. Albrecht,et al. Striate cortex responses to periodic patterns with and without the fundamental harmonics , 1981, The Journal of physiology.
[77] H. Wilson,et al. Perceived direction of moving two-dimensional patterns , 1990, Vision Research.
[78] C W Tyler,et al. Stereoscopic Depth Movement: Two Eyes Less Sensitive than One , 1971, Science.
[79] Leslie Welch,et al. The perception of moving plaids reveals two motion-processing stages , 1989, Nature.
[80] D. Hubel,et al. Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex , 1962, The Journal of physiology.
[81] D. Heeger. Normalization of cell responses in cat striate cortex , 1992, Visual Neuroscience.
[82] E H Adelson,et al. Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[83] David J. Fleet,et al. Computation of normal velocity from local phase information , 1989, Proceedings CVPR '89: IEEE Computer Society Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition.
[84] F Metelli,et al. The perception of transparency. , 1974, Scientific American.
[85] K. A. Stevens,et al. The Analogy between Stereo Depth and Brightness , 1989, Perception.
[86] David J. Fleet,et al. Stability of Phase Information , 1993, IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell..
[87] Eero P. Simoncelli,et al. A computational model for perception of two-dimensional pattern ve-locities , 1992 .
[88] C. Blakemore,et al. Evidence for disparity detecting neurones in the human visual system , 1972, The Journal of physiology.
[89] J. Ninio. Orientational versus Horizontal Disparity in the Stereoscopic Appreciation of Slant , 1985, Perception.
[90] A. T. Smith,et al. The influence of spatial frequency on perceived temporal frequency and perceived speed , 1990, Vision Research.
[91] Moirés Maintained Internally by Binocular Vision , 1978, Perception.
[92] A. Parker,et al. Stereo, surfaces and shape , 1991 .
[93] J Hirsch,et al. Limits of spatial-frequency discrimination as evidence of neural interpolation. , 1982, Journal of the Optical Society of America.
[94] P. Thompson. Perceived rate of movement depends on contrast , 1982, Vision Research.
[95] M. Treisman,et al. Relation between signal detectability theory and the traditional procedures for measuring sensory thresholds: Estimating d' from results given by the method of constant simuli. , 1966 .
[96] Mark A. Georgeson,et al. Temporal properties of spatial contrast vision , 1987, Vision Research.
[97] David J. Fleet. Measurement of image velocity , 1992 .
[98] C. Blakemore,et al. A second neural mechanism of binocular depth discrimination , 1972, The Journal of physiology.
[99] B. Julesz,et al. Independent Spatial-Frequency-Tuned Channels in Binocular Fusion and Rivalry , 1975 .
[100] H. Wilson,et al. Coarse spatial scales constrain the range of binocular fusion on fine scales. , 1991, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[101] D. G. Albrecht,et al. Visual cortical receptive fields in monkey and cat: Spatial and temporal phase transfer function , 1989, Vision Research.
[102] R. Heydt,et al. THE ROLE OF ORIENTATION DISPARITY IN STEREOSCOPIC PERCEPTION AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF BINOCULAR CORRESPONDENCE , 1981 .
[103] H. Wallach,et al. Two forms of retinal disparity , 1976 .
[104] John K. Tsotsos,et al. Techniques for disparity measurement , 1991, CVGIP Image Underst..
[105] G. Legge. A power law for contrast discrimination , 1981, Vision Research.
[106] Ian P. Howard,et al. Binocular fusion and rivalry , 1996 .
[107] Mary M. Conte,et al. Coherence and transparency of moving plaids composed of Fourier and non-Fourier gratings , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.
[108] Gregory C. DeAngelis,et al. Depth is encoded in the visual cortex by a specialized receptive field structure , 1991, Nature.
[109] Hans Knutsson,et al. Filtering and reconstruction in image processing , 1984 .
[110] H. Smallman. Fine-to-coarse scale disambiguation in stereopsis , 1995, Vision Research.
[111] Vision: Mechanisms for coding luminance patterns: are they really linear? , 1991 .
[112] Kenneth J. Ciuffreda. Binocular Vision and Stereopsis , 1998 .
[113] A. T. Smith,et al. Perceived speed and direction of complex gratings and plaids. , 1991, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[114] Hugh R. Wilson,et al. 10 – THE PERCEPTION OF FORM: Retina to Striate Cortex , 1989 .
[115] C. Blakemore,et al. Orientation Specificity and Spatial Selectivity in Human Vision , 1973, Perception.
[116] R. Blake,et al. How Contrast Affects Stereoacuity , 1988, Perception.
[117] S Marcelja,et al. Mathematical description of the responses of simple cortical cells. , 1980, Journal of the Optical Society of America.
[118] J P Frisby,et al. The Computation of Binocular Edges , 1980, Perception.
[119] S. Freguia. Researches in Binocular Vision. , 1950 .
[120] Casper J. Erkelens,et al. Temporal aspects of binocular slant perception , 1996, Vision Research.
[121] E. Adelson,et al. Phenomenal coherence of moving visual patterns , 1982, Nature.
[122] S. McKee,et al. Mechanisms underlying the anisotropy of stereoscopic tilt perception , 1990, Vision Research.
[123] A. W. Volkmann,et al. Physiologische Untersuchungen im Gebiete der Optik , 1863 .
[124] Kenneth N. Ogle,et al. INDUCED SIZE EFFECT: I. A NEW PHENOMENON IN BINOCULAR SPACE PERCEPTION ASSOCIATED WITH THE RELATIVE SIZES OF THE IMAGES OF THE TWO EYES , 1938 .
[125] H. Spitzer,et al. A complex-cell receptive-field model. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.
[126] E. Adelson,et al. Directionally selective complex cells and the computation of motion energy in cat visual cortex , 1992, Vision Research.
[127] D. Kersten. Transparency and the cooperative computation of scene attributes , 1991 .
[128] S. Fernberger. A figural after-effect in the third dimension of visual space. , 1948, The American journal of psychology.
[129] David J. Fleet,et al. Neural encoding of binocular disparity: Energy models, position shifts and phase shifts , 1996, Vision Research.
[130] T. D. Albright,et al. Transparency and coherence in human motion perception , 1990, Nature.
[131] E. Adelson,et al. The analysis of moving visual patterns , 1985 .
[132] F. Campbell,et al. The influence of spatial frequency and contrast on the perception of moving patterns , 1981, Vision Research.
[133] B. Rogers,et al. Anisotropies in the perception of stereoscopic surfaces: The role of orientation disparity , 1993, Vision Research.
[134] Andrew M. Derrington. Distortion products in geniculate X-cells: A physiological basis for masking by spatially modulated gratings? , 1987, Vision Research.
[135] P. Bressan,et al. Occlusion, transparency, and stereopsis: A new explanation for stereo capture , 1994, Vision Research.
[136] K. A. Stevens. Surface tilt (the direction of slant): A neglected psychophysical variable , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.
[137] M. J. Mayer,et al. Smooth frequency discrimination functions for foveal, high-contrast, mid spatial frequencies. , 1986, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[138] G. Sperling,et al. Drift-balanced random stimuli: a general basis for studying non-Fourier motion perception. , 1988, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[139] V. Ramachandran,et al. Transparency: Relation to Depth, Subjective Contours, Luminance, and Neon Color Spreading , 1990, Perception.
[140] Binocular Depth Perception , 1960, Nature.
[141] M. Georgeson,et al. Perceived contrast of gratings and plaids: Non-linear summation across oriented filters , 1994, Vision Research.
[142] Keith Langley,et al. Computational analysis of non-Fourier motion , 1994, Vision Research.
[143] D. Pollen,et al. Phase relationships between adjacent simple cells in the visual cortex. , 1981, Science.
[144] G. Poggio,et al. Binocular interaction and depth sensitivity in striate and prestriate cortex of behaving rhesus monkey. , 1977, Journal of neurophysiology.
[145] Andrea J. van Doorn,et al. Invariant Properties of the Motion Parallax Field due to the Movement of Rigid Bodies Relative to an Observer , 1975 .