The Role of the Size Maze and Learning Parameters in the Prefrontal Cortex Modeling Based in Minicolumns

Learning pathways in spatial navigation has been a subject of the literature in the last decade, one must bear about decision making and situation management. Column models were characterized few years ago and current implementations of the prefrontal brain cortex (PFC) simulated the rat behavior in a 3x3 maze given a Goal-Driven task. In this work, the simulation was adapted to study learning variables and goal task processing. The model was adapted to study different situations such a (1) 'µ' parameter value (for learning enhancement or degeneration) and different limits between a half and the entire amplitude of the threshold parameter, and (2) size of the maze (3x3, 3x4, 3x6 and 3x8 in tabulated simulations) related with the initial position of the rat and the goal condition (reward position). The initially position did not increment the average number of step to learn the way, but the when vertical size was increased to more than 4/3 the horizontal maze size, the number of steps was increased to learn the optimal pathway to reach to reward. Then, the larger size maze the more difficult to the PFC model to learn the optimal pathway and this was discussed in the current view of the entorhinal cortex and how this model process a different number of goals for a Goal-Driven task, especially considering modelling of adquisition and learning variables in the minicolumn model. A short discussion is extended about studies of situation management.

[1]  Carlos Mugruza-Vassallo Different regressors for linear modelling of ElectroEncephaloGraphic recordings in visual and auditory tasks , 2016, 2016 IEEE 13th International Conference on Wearable and Implantable Body Sensor Networks (BSN).

[2]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  An integrate-and-fire model of prefrontal cortex neuronal activity during performance of goal-directed decision making. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[3]  T. Hafting,et al.  Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex , 2005, Nature.

[4]  坂上 雅道 Encoding of behavioral significance of visual stimuli by primate prefrontal neurons: relation to relevant task conditions , 2000 .

[5]  M. Sakagami,et al.  Spatial selectivity of go/no-go neurons in monkey prefrontal cortex , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  E Courchesne,et al.  Reduced minicolumns in the frontal cortex of patients with autism , 2006, Neuropathology and applied neurobiology.

[7]  Hinako Toyama,et al.  The role of the prefrontal cortex in the go/no‐go task in humans: a positron emission tomography study , 2000 .

[8]  M. Vassallo,et al.  EEG and fMRI studies of the effects of stimulus properties on the control of attention , 2015 .

[9]  K. Weissenborn,et al.  Attention, Memory, and Cognitive Function in Hepatic Encephalopathy , 2005, Metabolic Brain Disease.

[10]  Michael E. Hasselmo,et al.  A Model of Prefrontal Cortical Mechanisms for Goal-directed Behavior , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Making Working Memory Work: A Computational Model of Learning in the Prefrontal Cortex and Basal Ganglia , 2006, Neural Computation.

[12]  G. Rousselet,et al.  Parametric study of EEG sensitivity to phase noise during face processing , 2008, BMC Neuroscience.

[13]  C. Summerfield,et al.  An information theoretical approach to prefrontal executive function , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[14]  E. Koechlin,et al.  The Architecture of Cognitive Control in the Human Prefrontal Cortex , 2003, Science.

[15]  Bhaskar D. Rao,et al.  Sparse Signal Recovery With Temporally Correlated Source Vectors Using Sparse Bayesian Learning , 2011, IEEE Journal of Selected Topics in Signal Processing.

[16]  Claire-Bénédicte Rivara Les cellules de Betz du cortex moteur primaire : analyse stéréologique et fonctionnelle , 2003 .

[17]  Carlos A. Mugruza-Vassallo,et al.  Database methodology for therapy evaluation in auditory schizophrenia disorder based on continuity evolution of symptoms , 2016, 2016 6th International Conference on Information Communication and Management (ICICM).

[18]  R. Haier,et al.  Topographic evoked potential mapping in obsessive-compulsive disorder: Evidence of frontal lobe dysfunction , 1989, Psychiatry Research.

[19]  M. Moser,et al.  Representation of Geometric Borders in the Entorhinal Cortex , 2008, Science.

[20]  Yasuyuki Taki,et al.  Regional gray matter density associated with emotional intelligence: Evidence from voxel‐based morphometry , 2011, Human brain mapping.