Parallel Distributed Networks Dissociate Episodic and Social Functions Within the Individual.

Association cortex is organized into large-scale distributed networks. One such network, the default network (DN), is linked to diverse forms of internal mentation, opening debate about whether shared or distinct anatomy supports multiple forms of cognition. Using within-individual analysis procedures that preserve idiosyncratic anatomical details, we probed whether multiple tasks from two domains - Episodic Projection and Theory of Mind (ToM) - rely upon the same or distinct networks. In an initial experiment (6 subjects, each scanned 4 times), we found evidence that Episodic Projection and ToM tasks activate separate regions distributed throughout cortex, with adjacent regions in parietal, temporal, prefrontal and midline zones. These distinctions were predicted by the hypothesis that the DN comprises two parallel, interdigitated networks. One network, linked to parahippocampal cortex (PHC), is preferentially recruited during Episodic Projection, including both remembering and imagining the future. A second juxtaposed network, which includes the temporoparietal junction (TPJ), is differentially engaged during multiple forms of ToM. In two prospectively-acquired independent experiments, we replicated and triplicated the dissociation (each with 6 subjects scanned 4 times). Furthermore, the dissociation was found in all zones when analyzed independently, including robustly in midline regions previously described as hubs. The TPJ-linked network is interwoven with the PHC-linked network across the cortex making clear why it is difficult to fully resolve the two networks in group-averaged or lower-resolution data. These results refine our understanding of the functional-anatomical organization of association cortex and raise fundamental questions about how specialization might arise in parallel, juxtaposed association networks.

[1]  M. Mesulam,et al.  From sensation to cognition. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[2]  Randy L. Buckner,et al.  The evolution of distributed association networks in the human brain , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[3]  D. Schacter,et al.  Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[4]  J. Perner,et al.  Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews Fractionating Theory of Mind: a Meta-analysis of Functional Brain Imaging Studies , 2022 .

[5]  Istvan Molnar-Szakacs,et al.  Watching social interactions produces dorsomedial prefrontal and medial parietal BOLD fMRI signal increases compared to a resting baseline , 2004, NeuroImage.

[6]  Randy L. Buckner,et al.  Parallel distributed networks resolved at high resolution reveal close juxtaposition of distinct regions , 2018, bioRxiv.

[7]  Evan M. Gordon,et al.  Functional System and Areal Organization of a Highly Sampled Individual Human Brain , 2015, Neuron.

[8]  R. N. Spreng,et al.  The default network and self‐generated thought: component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance , 2014, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[9]  Rebecca Saxe,et al.  Localizing Pain Matrix and Theory of Mind networks with both verbal and non-verbal stimuli , 2015, NeuroImage.

[10]  Ivan Toni,et al.  On the relationship between the “default mode network” and the “social brain” , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[11]  Marc Joliot,et al.  Brain activity at rest: a multiscale hierarchical functional organization. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  Rebecca Saxe,et al.  Contributions of episodic retrieval and mentalizing to autobiographical thought: Evidence from functional neuroimaging, resting-state connectivity, and fMRI meta-analyses , 2014, NeuroImage.

[13]  Mark Jenkinson,et al.  The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the Human Connectome Project , 2013, NeuroImage.

[14]  M M Mesulam,et al.  Large‐scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language, and memory , 1990, Annals of neurology.

[15]  Martial Van der Linden,et al.  Self-referential reflective activity and its relationship with rest: a PET study , 2005, NeuroImage.

[16]  Hans Knutsson,et al.  Cluster failure: Why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[17]  Mark W. Woolrich,et al.  Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL , 2004, NeuroImage.

[18]  Evan M. Gordon,et al.  Long-term neural and physiological phenotyping of a single human , 2015, Nature Communications.

[19]  R. Buckner,et al.  The brain’s default network: updated anatomy, physiology and evolving insights , 2019, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[20]  M. Livingstone,et al.  A hierarchical, retinotopic proto-organization of the primate visual system at birth , 2017, eLife.

[21]  Emile G. Bruneau,et al.  Distinct roles of the ‘Shared Pain’ and ‘Theory of Mind’ networks in processing others’ emotional suffering , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  Evan M. Gordon,et al.  Precision Functional Mapping of Individual Human Brains , 2017, Neuron.

[23]  R. Nathan Spreng,et al.  The Common Neural Basis of Autobiographical Memory, Prospection, Navigation, Theory of Mind, and the Default Mode: A Quantitative Meta-analysis , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[24]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  A critique of the use of the Kolmogorov‐Smirnov (KS) statistic for the analysis of BOLD fMRI data , 1998, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[25]  M. Raichle,et al.  Searching for a baseline: Functional imaging and the resting human brain , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[26]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images , 2001, Medical Image Anal..

[27]  Donald T. Stuss,et al.  Common and Unique Neural Correlates of Autobiographical Memory and Theory of Mind , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[28]  Joseph T. McGuire,et al.  Spectral partitioning identifies individual heterogeneity in the functional network topography of ventral and anterior medial prefrontal cortex , 2019, NeuroImage.

[29]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  Language-Selective and Domain-General Regions Lie Side by Side within Broca’s Area , 2012, Current Biology.

[30]  R. Saxe Uniquely human social cognition , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[31]  Rodrigo M. Braga,et al.  Parallel Interdigitated Distributed Networks within the Individual Estimated by Intrinsic Functional Connectivity , 2017, Neuron.

[32]  R. N. Spreng,et al.  The Future of Memory: Remembering, Imagining, and the Brain , 2012, Neuron.

[33]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  Social cognitive neuroscience: a review of core processes. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.

[34]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Reading fiction and reading minds: the role of simulation in the default network. , 2016, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[35]  Adrian W. Gilmore,et al.  Distinct subdivisions of human medial parietal cortex support recollection of people and places , 2019, eLife.

[36]  Daniel S. Margulies,et al.  Macroscale cortical organization and a default-like apex transmodal network in the marmoset monkey , 2019, Nature Communications.

[37]  R. Buckner,et al.  Functional-Anatomic Fractionation of the Brain's Default Network , 2010, Neuron.

[38]  Demis Hassabis,et al.  Imagine all the people: how the brain creates and uses personality models to predict behavior. , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[39]  Zachary C. Irving,et al.  Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: a dynamic framework , 2016, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[40]  D. Hassabis,et al.  Deconstructing episodic memory with construction , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[41]  Olaf Blanke,et al.  Brain system for mental orientation in space, time, and person , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[42]  William W. Graves,et al.  Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[43]  Steen Moeller,et al.  Evaluation of slice accelerations using multiband echo planar imaging at 3T , 2013, NeuroImage.

[44]  R. Nathan Spreng,et al.  Patterns of Brain Activity Supporting Autobiographical Memory, Prospection, and Theory of Mind, and Their Relationship to the Default Mode Network , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[45]  Bruce Fischl,et al.  Accurate and robust brain image alignment using boundary-based registration , 2009, NeuroImage.

[46]  Thomas L. Griffiths,et al.  Supplementary Information for Natural Speech Reveals the Semantic Maps That Tile Human Cerebral Cortex , 2022 .

[47]  T. Shallice From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure: The Cognitive Neuropsychology Approach , 1988 .

[48]  Timothy O. Laumann,et al.  Functional Network Organization of the Human Brain , 2011, Neuron.

[49]  John W. Harwell,et al.  Similar patterns of cortical expansion during human development and evolution , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[50]  A. Dale,et al.  Cortical Surface-Based Analysis II: Inflation, Flattening, and a Surface-Based Coordinate System , 1999, NeuroImage.

[51]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  New method for fMRI investigations of language: defining ROIs functionally in individual subjects. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[52]  Neal J. Cohen,et al.  Differential contributions of hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex to self-projection and self-referential processing , 2015, Neuropsychologia.

[53]  P. Goldman-Rakic Topography of cognition: parallel distributed networks in primate association cortex. , 1988, Annual review of neuroscience.

[54]  Julia M. Huntenburg,et al.  Large-Scale Gradients in Human Cortical Organization , 2018, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[55]  B. Levine,et al.  The functional neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: A meta-analysis , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[56]  R Saxe,et al.  People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.

[57]  Wen-Ming Luh,et al.  Multi-echo fMRI replication sample of autobiographical memory, prospection and theory of mind reasoning tasks , 2016, Scientific Data.

[58]  Evelina Fedorenko,et al.  A robust dissociation among the language, multiple demand, and default mode networks: Evidence from inter-region correlations in effect size , 2017, Neuropsychologia.

[59]  R. Buckner,et al.  Self-projection and the brain , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[60]  Xi-Nian Zuo,et al.  Spatial Topography of Individual-Specific Cortical Networks Predicts Human Cognition, Personality, and Emotion. , 2019, Cerebral cortex.

[61]  Timothy O. Laumann,et al.  Informatics and Data Mining Tools and Strategies for the Human Connectome Project , 2011, Front. Neuroinform..

[62]  Brian Levine,et al.  Theory of Mind Is Independent of Episodic Memory , 2007, Science.

[63]  D. Schacter,et al.  The Brain's Default Network , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[64]  Elizabeth Jefferies,et al.  Situating the default-mode network along a principal gradient of macroscale cortical organization , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[65]  Steen Moeller,et al.  Multiband multislice GE‐EPI at 7 tesla, with 16‐fold acceleration using partial parallel imaging with application to high spatial and temporal whole‐brain fMRI , 2010, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[66]  André J. W. van der Kouwe,et al.  Brain morphometry with multiecho MPRAGE , 2008, NeuroImage.

[67]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  Multiplexed Echo Planar Imaging for Sub-Second Whole Brain FMRI and Fast Diffusion Imaging , 2010, PloS one.

[68]  Margaret S Livingstone,et al.  Retinotopic Organization of Scene Areas in Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex , 2017, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[69]  Barbara G. Shinn-Cunningham,et al.  Short-Term Memory for Space and Time Flexibly Recruit Complementary Sensory-Biased Frontal Lobe Attention Networks , 2015, Neuron.

[70]  Rebecca Saxe,et al.  fMRI item analysis in a theory of mind task , 2011, NeuroImage.

[71]  A. Dale,et al.  Functional-Anatomic Correlates of Object Priming in Humans Revealed by Rapid Presentation Event-Related fMRI , 1998, Neuron.