Neural evidence for dissociable components of task-switching.

The ability to retrieve and flexibly switch between task rules is seen as an important component of cognitive control. It is often assumed that lateral prefrontal cortex (latPFC) is important for switching between rules. However, activation associated with rule-switching is less reliably observed in latPFC than in medial PFC (specifically, pre-supplementary motor area). In this study, we tested the hypothesis that medial PFC is important for reconfiguration of task sets, whereas latPFC is important for retrieving, maintaining and implementing relevant rules (i.e. rule representation). Twenty young adults participated in a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in which they determined the correct response to a target stimulus on the basis of an instructional cue. For bivalent targets, the appropriate response depended on the currently relevant rule. In contrast, univalent targets were always associated with the same response. Brain regions of interest were characterized according to their responsiveness to bivalent and univalent targets, on both rule-switch and rule-repetition trials. The data support the hypothesis that rule representation and task-set reconfiguration are separable cognitive processes, associated with dissociable neural activation in latPFC and medial PFC, respectively. Activation profiles of posterior parietal cortex, basal ganglia and rostrolateral PFC are also examined and discussed.

[1]  M. Rushworth,et al.  Complementary localization and lateralization of orienting and motor attention , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[2]  K. A. Hadland,et al.  Role of the human medial frontal cortex in task switching: a combined fMRI and TMS study. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[4]  Nachshon Meiran,et al.  Modelling cognitive control in task switching and ageing , 2001 .

[5]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Switching between spatial stimulus-response mappings: a developmental study of cognitive flexibility. , 2004, Developmental science.

[6]  R. Knight,et al.  Human prefrontal lesions increase distractibility to irrelevant sensory inputs , 1995, Neuroreport.

[7]  Marcel Brass,et al.  When the same response has different meanings: recoding the response meaning in the lateral prefrontal cortex , 2003, NeuroImage.

[8]  N. Meiran Modeling cognitive control in task-switching , 2000, Psychological research.

[9]  R. Passingham,et al.  Learning Arbitrary Visuomotor Associations: Temporal Dynamic of Brain Activity , 2001, NeuroImage.

[10]  Jeremy R. Reynolds,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Transient and Sustained Cognitive Control during Task Switching , 2003, Neuron.

[11]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  Task-Set Switching Deficits in Early-Stage Huntington's Disease: Implications for Basal Ganglia Function , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[12]  R. Passingham,et al.  Specialisation within the prefrontal cortex: the ventral prefrontal cortex and associative learning , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[14]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  BrainWeb: Online Interface to a 3D MRI Simulated Brain Database , 1997 .

[15]  M. Walton,et al.  Action sets and decisions in the medial frontal cortex , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[16]  A. Allport,et al.  Task switching and the measurement of “switch costs” , 2000, Psychological research.

[17]  S. Bunge How we use rules to select actions: A review of evidence from cognitive neuroscience , 2004, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[18]  T. Bussey,et al.  Role of prefrontal cortex in a network for arbitrary visuomotor mapping , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  T. Braver,et al.  The Role of Frontopolar Cortex in Subgoal Processing during Working Memory , 2002, NeuroImage.

[20]  G. D. Logan Task Switching , 2022 .

[21]  M. Brass,et al.  Decomposing Components of Task Preparation with Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[22]  J. Desmond,et al.  Functional Specialization for Semantic and Phonological Processing in the Left Inferior Prefrontal Cortex , 1999, NeuroImage.

[23]  P. Strick,et al.  Imaging the premotor areas , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[24]  E. Miller,et al.  Neural circuits subserving the retrieval and maintenance of abstract rules. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  T. Robbins,et al.  Contrasting Cortical and Subcortical Activations Produced by Attentional-Set Shifting and Reversal Learning in Humans , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  D. Alan Allport,et al.  SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .

[27]  E. Bizzi,et al.  The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1996 .

[28]  M. Gluck,et al.  Interactive memory systems in the human brain , 2001, Nature.

[29]  T. Robbins,et al.  Differential Responses in Human Striatum and Prefrontal Cortex to Changes in Object and Rule Relevance , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  T. Shallice,et al.  Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  B. Milner Effects of Different Brain Lesions on Card Sorting: The Role of the Frontal Lobes , 1963 .

[32]  A. Dove,et al.  Prefrontal cortex activation in task switching: an event-related fMRI study. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[33]  Eveline A. Crone,et al.  Retrieving rules for behavior from long-term memory , 2005, NeuroImage.

[34]  R. Passingham,et al.  Changes of cortico-striatal effective connectivity during visuomotor learning. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[35]  Tor D Wager,et al.  Neuroimaging studies of shifting attention: a meta-analysis , 2004, NeuroImage.

[36]  James B. Rowe,et al.  Parahippocampal Reactivation Signal at Retrieval after Interruption of Rehearsal , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  E. Miller,et al.  Task-specific neural activity in the primate prefrontal cortex. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  K. Berman,et al.  Fractionating the neural substrate of cognitive control processes , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[39]  C. Frith,et al.  Brain regions involved in prospective memory as determined by positron emission tomography , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Recovering Meaning Left Prefrontal Cortex Guides Controlled Semantic Retrieval , 2001, Neuron.

[41]  S. Los,et al.  On the origin of mixing costs: exploring information processing in pure and mixed blocks of trials , 1996 .

[42]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Anterior prefrontal cortex mediates rule learning in humans. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[43]  Ivan Toni,et al.  Prefrontal-basal ganglia pathways are involved in the learning of arbitrary visuomotor associations: a PET study , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[44]  E. Miller,et al.  Neural Activity in the Primate Prefrontal Cortex during Associative Learning , 1998, Neuron.

[45]  Robert T Knight,et al.  Both random and perseverative errors underlie WCST deficits in prefrontal patients , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[46]  Eliot Hazeltine,et al.  Dissociable Contributions of Prefrontal and Parietal Cortices to Response Selection , 2002, NeuroImage.

[47]  Nachshon Meiran,et al.  Reconfiguration of stimulus task sets and response task sets during task switching , 2000 .

[48]  Chantal E. Stern,et al.  The nucleus accumbens in monkeys (Macaca fascicularis) , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[49]  M. Brass,et al.  The role of the frontal cortex in task preparation. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[50]  Anthony D. Wagner,et al.  Cognitive Control, Semantic Memory, and Priming: Contributions from Prefrontal Cortex. , 2004 .

[51]  J. Desmond,et al.  Prefrontal regions involved in keeping information in and out of mind. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[52]  Bernhard Hommel,et al.  Control of stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance , 2002, Psychological research.

[53]  T. Robbins,et al.  Defining the Neural Mechanisms of Probabilistic Reversal Learning Using Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[54]  D. Stuss,et al.  Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in patients with focal frontal and posterior brain damage: effects of lesion location and test structure on separable cognitive processes , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[55]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Switching attention and resolving interference: fMRI measures of executive functions , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[56]  A M Dale,et al.  Optimal experimental design for event‐related fMRI , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[57]  John R. Anderson,et al.  The role of prefrontal cortex and posterior parietal cortex in task switching. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[58]  Nikolaus R. McFarland,et al.  Striatonigrostriatal Pathways in Primates Form an Ascending Spiral from the Shell to the Dorsolateral Striatum , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[59]  G. Logan,et al.  Clever homunculus: is there an endogenous act of control in the explicit task-cuing procedure? , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[60]  T. Paus Primate anterior cingulate cortex: Where motor control, drive and cognition interface , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[61]  R. Kliegl,et al.  Task-set switching and long-term memory retrieval. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[62]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Visual Working Memory in Prefrontal Cortex of the Macaque , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[63]  N. Meiran Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. , 1996 .

[64]  R. Passingham,et al.  Prefrontal interactions reflect future task operations , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.