Two are better than one: Infant language learning from video improves in the presence of peers
暂无分享,去创建一个
Patricia K. Kuhl | Adrian Garcia-Sierra | P. Kuhl | A. García-Sierra | S. Lytle | Sarah Roseberry Lytle
[1] R Näätänen,et al. Maturation of mismatch negativity in infants. , 1998, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[2] Lauren J. Myers,et al. Baby FaceTime: can toddlers learn from online video chat? , 2017, Developmental science.
[3] Amy I. Nathanson. Mediation of Children’s Television Viewing: Working Toward Conceptual Clarity and Common Understanding , 2001 .
[4] K. Eklund,et al. Event‐related brain potentials to change in rapidly presented acoustic stimuli in newborns , 1997 .
[5] Linda Polka,et al. The Conditioned head turn procedure as a method for testing infant speech perception , 1997 .
[6] M. Goldstein,et al. Social interaction shapes babbling: Testing parallels between birdsong and speech , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[7] Yan H. Yu,et al. Neural mismatch indices of vowel discrimination in monolingually and bilingually exposed infants: Does attention matter? , 2012, Neuroscience Letters.
[8] Yu-Lin Tzeng,et al. Mismatch responses to lexical tone, initial consonant, and vowel in Mandarin-speaking preschoolers , 2012, Neuropsychologia.
[9] Angela D. Friederici,et al. Brain Responses in 4-Month-Old Infants Are Already Language Specific , 2007, Current Biology.
[10] Valerie L. Shafer,et al. The development of English vowel perception in monolingual and bilingual infants: Neurophysiological correlates , 2011, J. Phonetics.
[11] Daniel R. Anderson,et al. Television and Very Young Children , 2005 .
[12] R. Näätänen. The perception of speech sounds by the human brain as reflected by the mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic equivalent (MMNm). , 2001, Psychophysiology.
[13] Rebekah A. Richert,et al. Just a talking book? Word learning from watching baby videos. , 2009, The British journal of developmental psychology.
[14] P. Kuhl,et al. Phonetic learning as a pathway to language: new data and native language magnet theory expanded (NLM-e) , 2008, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[15] Valerie L. Shafer,et al. Maturation of Mismatch Negativity in Typically Developing Infants and Preschool Children , 2002, Ear and hearing.
[16] J. Nunnally,et al. Exploratory Behavior and Human Development , 1974 .
[17] S. Dehaene,et al. Speed and cerebral correlates of syllable discrimination in infants , 1994, Nature.
[18] P. Leppänen,et al. Mismatch negativity (MMN) as a tool for investigating auditory discrimination and sensory memory in infants and children , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[19] A. Meltzoff,et al. The importance of eyes: how infants interpret adult looking behavior. , 2002, Developmental psychology.
[20] N. Kraus,et al. The time course of auditory perceptual learning: neurophysiological changes during speech‐sound training , 1998, Neuroreport.
[21] P. Kuhl,et al. Foreign-language experience in infancy: Effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[22] D. Oller. The emergence of the speech capacity , 2000 .
[23] Patricia K Kuhl,et al. Early Language Learning and Literacy: Neuroscience Implications for Education. , 2011, Mind, brain and education : the official journal of the International Mind, Brain, and Education Society.
[24] Geoffrey L. Cohen,et al. Mere belonging: the power of social connections. , 2012, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[25] M. Sabri,et al. The effects of digital filtering on mismatch negativity in wakefulness and slow‐wave sleep , 2002, Journal of sleep research.
[26] A. Bigelow. Infants' Response to Live and Replay Interactions with Self and Mother. , 1996 .
[27] R. Näätänen,et al. Development of language-specific phoneme representations in the infant brain , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.
[28] R. Näätänen. Attention and brain function , 1992 .
[29] A. Meltzoff,et al. The development of gaze following and its relation to language. , 2005, Developmental science.
[30] Andrew N Meltzoff,et al. Connecting the dots from infancy to childhood: a longitudinal study connecting gaze following, language, and explicit theory of mind. , 2015, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[31] P. Kuhl,et al. Perception of native and non-native affricate-fricative contrasts: cross-language tests on adults and infants. , 2006, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[32] Dare A. Baldwin,et al. Infants' ability to consult the speaker for clues to word reference , 1993, Journal of Child Language.
[33] M. Krcmar,et al. Can Toddlers Learn Vocabulary from Television? An Experimental Approach , 2007 .
[34] Gergely Csibra,et al. Recognizing Communicative Intentions in Infancy , 2010 .
[35] Dare A. Baldwin,et al. Infants' contribution to the achievement of joint reference. , 1991, Child development.
[36] R. Ilmoniemi,et al. Language-specific phoneme representations revealed by electric and magnetic brain responses , 1997, Nature.
[37] G Dehaene-Lambertz,et al. A phonological representation in the infant brain , 1998, Neuroreport.
[38] Patricia K Kuhl,et al. Relationships between quantity of language input and brain responses in bilingual and monolingual infants. , 2016, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[39] R. Näätänen,et al. Maturation of the auditory event-related potentials during the first year of life , 2002, Neuroreport.
[40] Philip J. Dunham,et al. Joint-Attentional States and Lexical Acquisition at 18 Months. , 1993 .
[41] Alexis R. Lauricella,et al. Contingent computer interactions for young children's object retrieval success , 2010 .
[42] Andrew P. King,et al. Mothers provide differential feedback to infants' prelinguistic sounds , 2006 .
[43] Andrew N Meltzoff,et al. Peer Imitation by Toddlers in Laboratory, Home, and Day-Care Contexts: Implications for Social Learning and Memory. , 1993, Developmental psychology.
[44] Daniel L. Schwartz,et al. Teachable Agents and the Protégé Effect: Increasing the Effort Towards Learning , 2009 .
[45] N. Cowan,et al. Mismatch negativity in children and adults, and effects of an attended task. , 2000, Psychophysiology.
[46] L. Trainor,et al. Changes in auditory cortex and the development of mismatch negativity between 2 and 6 months of age. , 2003, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[47] Georgene L. Troseth,et al. Third-party social interaction and word learning from video. , 2011, Child development.
[48] Yu-Lin Tzeng,et al. The Development of Mismatch Responses to Mandarin Lexical Tones in Early Infancy , 2013, Developmental neuropsychology.
[49] Samu Taulu,et al. Speech discrimination in 11-month-old bilingual and monolingual infants: a magnetoencephalography study. , 2017, Developmental science.
[50] P. Kuhl. Is speech learning 'gated' by the social brain? , 2007, Developmental science.
[51] The Social Reach , 2016, Psychological science.
[52] Heather L. Kirkorian,et al. Toddlers' Word Learning From Contingent and Noncontingent Video on Touch Screens. , 2016, Child development.
[53] Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al. Putting Education in “Educational” Apps , 2015, Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society.
[54] K. Reinikainen,et al. The mismatch negativity to changes in speech sounds at the age of three months , 1997 .
[55] Hsin-Chi Wu,et al. Feature-specific transition from positive mismatch response to mismatch negativity in early infancy: mismatch responses to vowels and initial consonants. , 2015, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[56] D. Brandeis,et al. Development of the automatic mismatch response: from frontal positivity in kindergarten children to the mismatch negativity , 2003, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[57] B. Ryalls,et al. Infant Imitation of Peer and Adult Models: Evidence for a Peer Model Advantage , 2000 .
[58] M. Špinka,et al. Decoding of Baby Calls: Can Adult Humans Identify the Eliciting Situation from Emotional Vocalizations of Preverbal Infants? , 2015, PloS one.
[59] P. Kuhl,et al. Brain potentials to native and non-native speech contrasts in 7- and 11-month-old American infants. , 2005, Developmental science.
[60] Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al. Skype me! Socially contingent interactions help toddlers learn language. , 2014, Child development.
[61] K. Hirsh-Pasek,et al. Live action: can young children learn verbs from video? , 2009, Child development.
[62] Toshiaki Imada,et al. Effects of language experience: Neural commitment to language-specific auditory patterns , 2005, NeuroImage.
[63] K. Reinikainen,et al. Mismatch negativity indicates vowel discrimination in newborns , 1995, Hearing Research.