An illusion predicted by V1 population activity implicates cortical topography in shape perception

Mammalian primary visual cortex (V1) is topographically organized such that the pattern of neural activation in V1 reflects the location and spatial extent of visual elements in the retinal image, but it is unclear whether this organization contributes to visual perception. We combined computational modeling, voltage-sensitive dye imaging (VSDI) in behaving monkeys and behavioral measurements in humans to investigate whether the large-scale topography of V1 population responses influences shape judgments. Specifically, we used a computational model to design visual stimuli that had the same physical shape, but were predicted to elicit variable V1 response spread. We confirmed these predictions with VSDI. Finally, we designed a behavioral task in which human observers judged the shapes of these stimuli and found that their judgments were systematically distorted by the spread of V1 activity. This illusion suggests that the topographic pattern of neural population responses in visual cortex contributes to visual perception.

[1]  E H Adelson,et al.  Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[2]  Serge O. Dumoulin,et al.  Cortical specialization for concentric shape processing , 2007, Vision Research.

[3]  M. Morgan,et al.  Observers can voluntarily shift their psychometric functions without losing sensitivity , 2011, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[4]  R F Hess,et al.  Relationship between facilitation at threshold and suprathreshold contour integration. , 1998, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[5]  D. Hubel,et al.  Ferrier lecture - Functional architecture of macaque monkey visual cortex , 1977, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[6]  Leonard E. White,et al.  Vision and Cortical Map Development , 2007, Neuron.

[7]  Wolfgang Gawronski,et al.  Analytic properties of noncentral distributions , 2003, Appl. Math. Comput..

[8]  M. Paradiso,et al.  A theory for the use of visual orientation information which exploits the columnar structure of striate cortex , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[9]  Amiram Grinvald,et al.  VSDI: a new era in functional imaging of cortical dynamics , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[10]  D. L. Adams,et al.  A Precise Retinotopic Map of Primate Striate Cortex Generated from the Representation of Angioscotomas , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  R. Vautin,et al.  Magnification factor and receptive field size in foveal striate cortex of the monkey , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  M. A. Repucci,et al.  Spatial Structure and Symmetry of Simple-Cell Receptive Fields in Macaque Primary Visual Cortex , 2002 .

[13]  S. Jay Electroventilation. New interest in an old idea. , 1989, Chest.

[14]  D. Kersten,et al.  Attention-Dependent Representation of a Size Illusion in Human V1 , 2008, Current Biology.

[15]  E. Switkes,et al.  Functional anatomy of macaque striate cortex. III. Color , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[16]  D. Samuel Schwarzkopf,et al.  The surface area of human V1 predicts the subjective experience of object size , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[17]  E. Seidemann,et al.  Dynamics of Depolarization and Hyperpolarization in the Frontal Cortex and Saccade Goal , 2002, Science.

[18]  U. Polat,et al.  Collinear stimuli regulate visual responses depending on cell's contrast threshold , 1998, Nature.

[19]  David J. Field,et al.  Contour integration by the human visual system: Evidence for a local “association field” , 1993, Vision Research.

[20]  D. Ringach On the Origin of the Functional Architecture of the Cortex , 2007, PloS one.

[21]  D. Kersten,et al.  The representation of perceived angular size in human primary visual cortex , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[22]  Robert O. Duncan,et al.  Cortical Magnification within Human Primary Visual Cortex Correlates with Acuity Thresholds , 2003, Neuron.

[23]  B. Wandell,et al.  Visual Field Maps in Human Cortex , 2007, Neuron.

[24]  J. Nachmias Shape and size discrimination compared , 2011, Vision Research.

[25]  B. C. Motter Central V4 Receptive Fields Are Scaled by the V1 Cortical Magnification and Correspond to a Constant-Sized Sampling of the V1 Surface , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  M. Morgan,et al.  Observers can voluntarily shift their psychometric functions without losing sensitivity , 2012 .

[27]  Daniel L Adams,et al.  The cortical column: a structure without a function , 2005, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[28]  G. Blasdel,et al.  Functional Retinotopy of Monkey Visual Cortex , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  D. Regan,et al.  Two-dimensional Aspect Ratio Discrimination for Shape Defined by Orientation Texture , 1996, Vision Research.

[30]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  The visual field representation in striate cortex of the macaque monkey: Asymmetries, anisotropies, and individual variability , 1984, Vision Research.

[31]  H. Wilson,et al.  Detection of global structure in Glass patterns: implications for form vision , 1998, Vision Research.

[32]  E. Seidemann,et al.  Optimal temporal decoding of neural population responses in a reaction-time visual detection task. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  C. Gilbert,et al.  Improvement in visual sensitivity by changes in local context: Parallel studies in human observers and in V1 of alert monkeys , 1995, Neuron.

[34]  Eyal Seidemann,et al.  The relationship between voltage-sensitive dye imaging signals and spiking activity of neural populations in primate V1. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  R. L. de Valois,et al.  Relationship between spatial-frequency and orientation tuning of striate-cortex cells. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[36]  D. Regan,et al.  Shape discrimination and the judgement of perfect symmetry: Dissociation of shape from size , 1992, Vision Research.

[37]  N. Swindale,et al.  Receptive field and orientation scatter studied by tetrode recordings in cat area 17 , 1999, Visual Neuroscience.

[38]  Dario L. Ringach,et al.  Link between orientation and retinotopic maps in primary visual cortex , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[39]  E. Seidemann,et al.  Optimal decoding of correlated neural population responses in the primate visual cortex , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[40]  D. Chklovskii,et al.  Maps in the brain: what can we learn from them? , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[41]  Eyal Seidemann,et al.  Uniform spatial spread of population activity in primate parafoveal V1. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  E. Switkes,et al.  Functional anatomy of macaque striate cortex. II. Retinotopic organization , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[43]  James T. Mcllwain Point images in the visual system: new interest in an old idea , 1986, Trends in Neurosciences.

[44]  David J Heeger,et al.  Rapid and precise retinotopic mapping of the visual cortex obtained by voltage-sensitive dye imaging in the behaving monkey. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[45]  S. Dumoulin,et al.  The Relationship between Cortical Magnification Factor and Population Receptive Field Size in Human Visual Cortex: Constancies in Cortical Architecture , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[46]  Tobias Teichert,et al.  Scale-invariance of receptive field properties in primary visual cortex , 2007, BMC Neuroscience.

[47]  Minami Ito,et al.  Representation of Angles Embedded within Contour Stimuli in Area V2 of Macaque Monkeys , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  U. Polat,et al.  Lateral interactions between spatial channels: Suppression and facilitation revealed by lateral masking experiments , 1993, Vision Research.

[49]  C. Connor,et al.  Population coding of shape in area V4 , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[50]  D. Regan,et al.  Shape discrimination for rectangles defined by disparity alone, by disparity plus luminance and by disparity plus motion , 1994, Vision Research.

[51]  Wei Ji Ma,et al.  Bayesian inference with probabilistic population codes , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[52]  H Sompolinsky,et al.  Simple models for reading neuronal population codes. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[53]  T. Wiesel,et al.  Functional architecture of macaque monkey visual cortex , 1977 .

[54]  D. G. Albrecht,et al.  Spatial frequency selectivity of cells in macaque visual cortex , 1982, Vision Research.

[55]  Arnulf B. A. Graf,et al.  Decoding the activity of neuronal populations in macaque primary visual cortex , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[56]  J. Hegdé,et al.  A comparative study of shape representation in macaque visual areas v2 and v4. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.