Repetition-related reductions in neural activity reveal component processes of mental simulation.

In everyday life, people adaptively prepare for the future by simulating dynamic events about impending interactions with people, objects and locations. Previous research has consistently demonstrated that a distributed network of frontal-parietal-temporal brain regions supports this ubiquitous mental activity. Nonetheless, little is known about the manner in which specific regions of this network contribute to component features of future simulation. In two experiments, we used a functional magnetic resonance (fMR)-repetition suppression paradigm to demonstrate that distinct frontal-parietal-temporal regions are sensitive to processing the scenarios or what participants imagined was happening in an event (e.g., medial prefrontal, posterior cingulate, temporal-parietal and middle temporal cortices are sensitive to the scenarios associated with future social events), people (medial prefrontal cortex), objects (inferior frontal and premotor cortices) and locations (posterior cingulate/retrosplenial, parahippocampal and posterior parietal cortices) that typically constitute simulations of personal future events. This pattern of results demonstrates that the neural substrates of these component features of event simulations can be reliably identified in the context of a task that requires participants to simulate complex, everyday future experiences.

[1]  Russell A. Epstein Parahippocampal and retrosplenial contributions to human spatial navigation , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[2]  K. Szpunar,et al.  Contextual processing in episodic future thought. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[3]  R. Buckner,et al.  Functional-Anatomic Fractionation of the Brain's Default Network , 2010, Neuron.

[4]  John D. E. Gabrieli,et al.  Neural Correlates of Auditory Repetition Priming: Reduced fMRI Activation in the Auditory Cortex , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[5]  R. N. Spreng,et al.  The Future of Memory: Remembering, Imagining, and the Brain , 2012, Neuron.

[6]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  Repetition and the brain: neural models of stimulus-specific effects , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[7]  Boris Suchan,et al.  Foreseeing the future: Occurrence probability of imagined future events modulates hippocampal activation , 2009, Hippocampus.

[8]  F. Van Overwalle Social cognition and the brain: A meta‐analysis , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[9]  D. Schacter,et al.  The Hippocampus and Imagining the Future: Where Do We Stand? , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[10]  Martial Van der Linden,et al.  Predicting the phenomenology of episodic future thoughts , 2012, Consciousness and Cognition.

[11]  Eleanor A Maguire,et al.  A New Role for the Parahippocampal Cortex in Representing Space , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  D. Schacter,et al.  Repetition priming influences distinct brain systems: evidence from task-evoked data and resting-state correlations. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  K. Szpunar Episodic Future Thought , 2010, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[14]  Michael D. Rugg,et al.  Decrements in Hippocampal Activity with Item Repetition during Continuous Recognition: An fMRI Study , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[15]  F. Mayor Memory of the future , 1995 .

[16]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  The Mind's Eye—Precuneus Activation in Memory-Related Imagery , 1995, NeuroImage.

[17]  D. Schacter,et al.  Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[18]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Precuneus shares intrinsic functional architecture in humans and monkeys , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[19]  D. Schacter,et al.  Get real: effects of repeated simulation and emotion on the perceived plausibility of future experiences. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[20]  Philip A. Kragel,et al.  Dynamic neural networks supporting memory retrieval , 2011, NeuroImage.

[21]  D. Schacter,et al.  Episodic Simulation of Future Events , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[22]  Andrew P. Yonelinas,et al.  Adaptation to cognitive context and item information in the medial temporal lobes , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[23]  Demis Hassabis,et al.  Imagine all the people: how the brain creates and uses personality models to predict behavior. , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[24]  D. Hassabis,et al.  Using Imagination to Understand the Neural Basis of Episodic Memory , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  R. Nathan Spreng,et al.  The Common Neural Basis of Autobiographical Memory, Prospection, Navigation, Theory of Mind, and the Default Mode: A Quantitative Meta-analysis , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Repetition suppression of ventromedial prefrontal activity during judgments of self and others , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[27]  Masato Taira,et al.  Neural enhancement and attenuation induced by repetitive recall , 2011, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[28]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[29]  D. Schacter Adaptive constructive processes and the future of memory. , 2012, The American psychologist.

[30]  D. Schacter,et al.  Reductions in cortical activity during priming , 2007, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[31]  L. Davachi,et al.  Category‐specificity in the human medial temporal lobe cortex , 2009, Hippocampus.

[32]  R. Hari,et al.  Brain basis of human social interaction: from concepts to brain imaging. , 2009, Physiological reviews.

[33]  R. Buckner,et al.  Evidence for the Default Network's Role in Spontaneous Cognition , 2010 .

[34]  Ana Raposo,et al.  Contributions of frontopolar cortex to judgments about self, others and relations. , 2011, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[35]  Alana T. Wong,et al.  Remembering the past and imagining the future: Common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  D. Ingvar “Hyperfrontal” distribution of the cerebral grey matter flow in resting wakefulness; on the functional anatomy of the conscious state , 1979, Acta neurologica Scandinavica.

[37]  D. Ingvar "Memory of the future": an essay on the temporal organization of conscious awareness. , 1985, Human neurobiology.

[38]  A. Cavanna,et al.  The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[39]  Jessica R. Andrews-Hanna,et al.  The Brain’s Default Network and Its Adaptive Role in Internal Mentation , 2012, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[40]  M. Corballis,et al.  The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? , 2007, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[41]  R. Nathan Spreng,et al.  The Fallacy of a “Task-Negative” Network , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[42]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Common Blood Flow Changes across Visual Tasks: II. Decreases in Cerebral Cortex , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[43]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Default network activity, coupled with the frontoparietal control network, supports goal-directed cognition , 2010, NeuroImage.

[44]  K. Szpunar,et al.  Neural substrates of envisioning the future , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[45]  M. Corballis,et al.  Mental time travel and the evolution of the human mind. , 1997, Genetic, social, and general psychology monographs.

[46]  J. Haxby,et al.  Neural response to the visual familiarity of faces , 2006, Brain Research Bulletin.

[47]  D. Schacter,et al.  The Brain's Default Network , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[48]  Steven L. Small,et al.  Repetition Suppression for Spoken Sentences and the Effect of Task Demands , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.