Improved Probabilistic Inference as a General Learning Mechanism with Action Video Games

Action video game play benefits performance in an array of sensory, perceptual, and attentional tasks that go well beyond the specifics of game play [1-9]. That a training regimen may induce improvements in so many different skills is notable because the majority of studies on training-induced learning report improvements on the trained task but limited transfer to other, even closely related, tasks ([10], but see also [11-13]). Here we ask whether improved probabilistic inference may explain such broad transfer. By using a visual perceptual decision making task [14, 15], the present study shows for the first time that action video game experience does indeed improve probabilistic inference. A neural model of this task [16] establishes how changing a single parameter, namely the strength of the connections between the neural layer providing the momentary evidence and the layer integrating the evidence over time, captures improvements in action-gamers behavior. These results were established in a visual, but also in a novel auditory, task, indicating generalization across modalities. Thus, improved probabilistic inference provides a general mechanism for why action video game playing enhances performance in a wide variety of tasks. In addition, this mechanism may serve as a signature of training regimens that are likely to produce transfer of learning.

[1]  M. Stone Models for choice-reaction time , 1960 .

[2]  B. Dosher,et al.  The dynamics of perceptual learning: an incremental reweighting model. , 2005, Psychological review.

[3]  U. Polat,et al.  Enhancing the contrast sensitivity function through action video game training , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[4]  James L. McClelland,et al.  The time course of perceptual choice: the leaky, competing accumulator model. , 2001, Psychological review.

[5]  Robert A Jacobs,et al.  Adaptive precision pooling of model neuron activities predicts the efficiency of human visual learning. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[6]  M. Fahle Perceptual learning: specificity versus generalization , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[7]  Julie I. Cohen,et al.  Effect of action video games on the spatial distribution of visuospatial attention. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  Donald Laming,et al.  Information theory of choice-reaction times , 1968 .

[9]  J. Wolfowitz,et al.  Optimum Character of the Sequential Probability Ratio Test , 1948 .

[10]  Angela J. Yu,et al.  Dynamics of attentional selection under conflict: toward a rational Bayesian account. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  P. Greenfield,et al.  Action video games and informal education: Effects on strategies for dividing visual attention , 1994 .

[12]  K. Zilles,et al.  Polymodal Motion Processing in Posterior Parietal and Premotor Cortex A Human fMRI Study Strongly Implies Equivalencies between Humans and Monkeys , 2001, Neuron.

[13]  A. Mizuno,et al.  A change of the leading player in flow Visualization technique , 2006, J. Vis..

[14]  C. S. Green,et al.  Action video game modifies visual selective attention , 2003, Nature.

[15]  Timothy D. Hanks,et al.  Probabilistic Population Codes for Bayesian Decision Making , 2008, Neuron.

[16]  George A. Alvarez,et al.  Explaining human multiple object tracking as resource-constrained approximate inference in a dynamic probabilistic model , 2009, NIPS.

[17]  Louis D. Matzel,et al.  Working Memory Training Promotes General Cognitive Abilities in Genetically Heterogeneous Mice , 2010, Current Biology.

[18]  Anthony J. Movshon,et al.  Optimal representation of sensory information by neural populations , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[19]  Roger W Li,et al.  Perceptual learning improves efficiency by re-tuning the decision 'template' for position discrimination , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[20]  Jacobs Robert,et al.  Adaptive precision pooling of model neuron activities predicts efficiency of human visual learning , 2009 .

[21]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Microstimulation of visual cortex affects the speed of perceptual decisions , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[22]  Peter Dayan,et al.  Fast Population Coding , 2007, Neural Computation.

[23]  J. Pratt,et al.  Playing an Action Video Game Reduces Gender Differences in Spatial Cognition , 2007, Psychological science.

[24]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  The Diffusion Decision Model: Theory and Data for Two-Choice Decision Tasks , 2008, Neural Computation.

[25]  Daphna Weinshall,et al.  Mechanisms of generalization in perceptual learning , 1998, Vision Research.

[26]  C. Law,et al.  Reinforcement learning can account for associative and perceptual learning on a visual decision task , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[27]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Response of Neurons in the Lateral Intraparietal Area during a Combined Visual Discrimination Reaction Time Task , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  E. Bialystok Effect of bilingualism and computer video game experience on the Simon task. , 2006, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[29]  Barbara Anne Dosher,et al.  Task precision at transfer determines specificity of perceptual learning. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[30]  Wei Ji Ma,et al.  Bayesian inference with probabilistic population codes , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  R. W. Allan,et al.  Psychology of Computer Use: XII. Videogame Play: Human Reaction Time to Visual Stimuli , 1989 .

[32]  Marisa Carrasco,et al.  Speed of visual processing increases with eccentricity , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[33]  Barbara Anne Dosher,et al.  Empirical approaches to information processing: Speed-accuracy tradeoff functions or reaction time — a reply , 1979 .

[34]  A. Pouget,et al.  Efficient computation and cue integration with noisy population codes , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[35]  M. Shadlen,et al.  The effect of stimulus strength on the speed and accuracy of a perceptual decision. , 2005, Journal of vision.

[36]  S. Klein,et al.  Complete Transfer of Perceptual Learning across Retinal Locations Enabled by Double Training , 2008, Current Biology.

[37]  C S Green,et al.  Action-Video-Game Experience Alters the Spatial Resolution of Vision , 2007, Psychological science.

[38]  C. S. Green,et al.  Enumeration versus multiple object tracking: the case of action video game players , 2006, Cognition.

[39]  J. Pratt,et al.  The effects of action video game experience on the time course of inhibition of return and the efficiency of visual search. , 2005, Acta psychologica.

[40]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  Psychology and neurobiology of simple decisions , 2004, Trends in Neurosciences.

[41]  Jonas Persson,et al.  Gaining Control: Training Executive Function and Far Transfer of the Ability to Resolve Interference [retracted] , 2008, Psychological science.

[42]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  Modeling Response Times for Two-Choice Decisions , 1998 .

[43]  J D Cohen,et al.  A network model of catecholamine effects: gain, signal-to-noise ratio, and behavior. , 1990, Science.

[44]  J. Andel Sequential Analysis , 2022, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[45]  C. Law,et al.  Neural correlates of perceptual learning in a sensory-motor, but not a sensory, cortical area , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[46]  M. Shadlen,et al.  A role for neural integrators in perceptual decision making. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[47]  Wei Ji Ma,et al.  No capacity limit in attentional tracking: evidence for probabilistic inference under a resource constraint. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[48]  Susanne M. Jaeggi,et al.  Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory: a meta-analysis , 2008, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.