The Effects of Aging on the Neural Basis of Implicit Associative Learning in a Probabilistic Triplets Learning Task

Few studies have investigated how aging influences the neural basis of implicit associative learning, and available evidence is inconclusive. One emerging behavioral pattern is that age differences increase with practice, perhaps reflecting the involvement of different brain regions with training. Many studies report hippocampal involvement early on with learning becoming increasingly dependent on the caudate with practice. We tested the hypothesis that the contribution of these regions to learning changes with age because of differential age-related declines in the striatum and hippocampi. We assessed age-related differences in brain activation during implicit associative learning using the Triplets Learning Task. Over three event-related fMRI runs, 11 younger and 12 healthy older adults responded to only the third (target) stimulus in sequences of three stimuli (“triplets”) by corresponding key press. Unbeknown to participants, the first stimulus' location predicted one target location for 80% of trials and another target location for 20% of trials. Both age groups learned associative regularities but differences in favor of the younger adults emerged with practice. The neural basis of learning (response to predictability) was examined by identifying regions that showed a greater response to triplets that occurred more frequently. Both age groups recruited the hippocampus early, but with training, the younger adults recruited their caudate whereas the older adults continued to rely on their hippocampus. This pattern enables older adults to maintain near-young levels of performance early in training, but not later, and adds to evidence that implicit associative learning is supported by different brain networks in younger and older adults.

[1]  Benjamin O. Turner,et al.  Cortical and basal ganglia contributions to habit learning and automaticity , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[2]  B. Meier,et al.  Incidental sequence learning across the lifespan , 2012, Cognition.

[3]  Daphna Shohamy,et al.  Strategies in probabilistic categorization: results from a new way of analyzing performance. , 2006, Learning & memory.

[4]  R. Faragher,et al.  Insights into CNS ageing from animal models of senescence , 2012, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[5]  B. Knowlton,et al.  An implicit learning task activates medial temporal lobe in patients with Parkinson's disease. , 2004, Behavioral neuroscience.

[6]  Chandan J. Vaidya,et al.  Caudate Resting Connectivity Predicts Implicit Probabilistic Sequence Learning , 2013, Brain Connect..

[7]  M. Naveh-Benjamin,et al.  Differential effects of age on item and associative measures of memory: a meta-analysis. , 2008, Psychology and aging.

[8]  James H. Howard,et al.  Implicit sequence learning without motor sequencing in young and old adults , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  Lars Bertram,et al.  Effects of aging and dopamine genotypes on the emergence of explicit memory during sequence learning , 2013, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  C. Vaidya,et al.  Dopamine transporter genotype predicts implicit sequence learning , 2011, Behavioural Brain Research.

[11]  Jennifer C. Romano,et al.  Two Forms of Implicit Learning in Young Adults with Dyslexia , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[12]  Chandan J. Vaidya,et al.  White matter integrity correlates of implicit sequence learning in healthy aging , 2011, Neurobiology of Aging.

[13]  M. Gluck,et al.  Probabilistic classification learning in amnesia. , 1994, Learning & memory.

[14]  D. Head,et al.  Differential aging of the medial temporal lobe , 2004, Neurology.

[15]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  Models of Implicit learning , 2003 .

[16]  J. Raaijmakers,et al.  Similar network activated by young and old adults during the acquisition of a motor sequence , 2003, Neurobiology of Aging.

[17]  F. Craik,et al.  The handbook of aging and cognition , 1992 .

[18]  D. Howard Dissociable Forms of Implicit Learning in Aging , 2011 .

[19]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  Processing abstract sequence structure: learning without knowing, or knowing without learning? , 2005, Psychological research.

[20]  D. Howard,et al.  Does a simultaneous memory load affect older and younger adults’ implicit associative learning? , 2014, Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition.

[21]  Sterling C. Johnson,et al.  Associative Learning Over Trials Activates the Hippocampus in Healthy Elderly but not Mild Cognitive Impairment , 2008, Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition.

[22]  C. Stern,et al.  An fMRI Study of the Role of the Medial Temporal Lobe in Implicit and Explicit Sequence Learning , 2003, Neuron.

[23]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  Effects of Age and Practice in Sequence Learning a Graded Account of Aging, Learning and Control , 2008 .

[24]  J. H. Howard,et al.  Impaired implicit sequence learning in children with developmental dyslexia. , 2013, Research in developmental disabilities.

[25]  Cheryl L. Dahle,et al.  Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: general trends, individual differences and modifiers. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[26]  Andrew J. Kelly,et al.  Implicit learning of predictive relationships in three-element visual sequences by young and old adults. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[27]  W. T. Maddox,et al.  A quantitative model-based approach to examining aging effects on information-integration category learning. , 2004, Psychology and aging.

[28]  Richard N. Aslin,et al.  The neural correlates of statistical learning in a word segmentation task: An fMRI study , 2013, Brain and Language.

[29]  N. Dennis,et al.  Event Timing and Age Deficits in Higher-Order Sequence Learning , 2007, Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition.

[30]  N. Tzourio-Mazoyer,et al.  Automated Anatomical Labeling of Activations in SPM Using a Macroscopic Anatomical Parcellation of the MNI MRI Single-Subject Brain , 2002, NeuroImage.

[31]  Naftali Raz,et al.  Neuroanatomical and cognitive correlates of adult age differences in acquisition of a perceptual‐motor skill , 2000, Microscopy research and technique.

[32]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Hippocampal and neocortical activation during repetitive encoding in older persons , 2006, Neurobiology of Aging.

[33]  J. H. Howard,et al.  Age differences in implicit learning of higher order dependencies in serial patterns. , 1997, Psychology and aging.

[34]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Functional Mapping of Sequence Learning in Normal Humans , 1995, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[35]  N. Burgess,et al.  Complementary memory systems: competition, cooperation and compensation , 2005, Trends in Neurosciences.

[36]  Roberto Cabeza,et al.  Age-related dedifferentiation of learning systems: an fMRI study of implicit and explicit learning , 2011, Neurobiology of Aging.

[37]  A. Karni,et al.  Learning perceptual skills: behavioral probes into adult cortical plasticity , 1997, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[38]  D. Howard,et al.  Adult age differences in learning on a sequentially cued prediction task. , 2014, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[39]  A. Reber Implicit learning of synthetic languages: The role of instructional set. , 1976 .

[40]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  Insights into the ageing mind: a view from cognitive neuroscience , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[41]  C. Myers,et al.  Learning and Generalization in Healthy Aging: Implication for Frontostriatal and Hippocampal Function , 2012, Cognitive and behavioral neurology : official journal of the Society for Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology.

[42]  M. Gluck,et al.  Basal ganglia and dopamine contributions to probabilistic category learning , 2008, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[43]  J. Doyon Motor sequence learning and movement disorders , 2008, Current opinion in neurology.

[44]  A M Dale,et al.  Optimal experimental design for event‐related fMRI , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[45]  Lars Bäckman,et al.  Implicit Learning in Aging: Extant Patterns and New Directions , 2009, Neuropsychology Review.

[46]  Edith V. Sullivan,et al.  Preservation of hippocampal volume throughout adulthood in healthy men and women , 2005, Neurobiology of Aging.

[47]  Lars Bäckman,et al.  Activation in striatum and medial temporal lobe during sequence learning in younger and older adults: Relations to performance , 2010, NeuroImage.

[48]  M. Chun,et al.  Memory deficits for implicit contextual information in amnesic subjects with hippocampal damage , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[49]  I. Bennett,et al.  Age-related differences in implicit learning of subtle third-order sequential structure. , 2007, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[50]  Timothy A. Salthouse,et al.  A framework for analyzing and interpreting differential aging patterns: Application to three measures of implicit learning , 1999 .

[51]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Que PASA? The posterior-anterior shift in aging. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[52]  Jean-Luc Anton,et al.  Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox , 2010 .

[53]  M. Gluck,et al.  Neural Mechanisms Underlying Probabilistic Category Learning in Normal Aging , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[54]  M. Gluck,et al.  Interactive memory systems in the human brain , 2001, Nature.

[55]  K. Henke A model for memory systems based on processing modes rather than consciousness , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[56]  Marvin M. Chun,et al.  Neural Evidence of Statistical Learning: Efficient Detection of Visual Regularities Without Awareness , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[57]  Faith M. Gunning-Dixon,et al.  Differential aging of the human striatum: longitudinal evidence. , 2003, AJNR. American journal of neuroradiology.

[58]  D. Howard,et al.  Implicit sequence learning: effects of level of structure, adult age, and extended practice. , 2004, Psychology and aging.

[59]  Catherine E. Myers,et al.  Gateway to Memory: An Introduction to Neural Network Modeling of the Hippocampus and Learning , 2000 .

[60]  Peter A. Frensch,et al.  One concept, multiple meanings: On how to define the concept of implicit learning. , 1998 .

[61]  M. Nissen,et al.  Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[62]  D. Howard,et al.  Age differences in implicit learning of probabilistic unstructured sequences. , 2011, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[63]  W. Todd Maddox,et al.  Rule-based and information-integration category learning in normal aging , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[64]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Competition among multiple memory systems: converging evidence from animal and human brain studies , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[65]  Cameron S. Carter,et al.  Prefrontal and striatal activation in elderly subjects during concurrent implicit and explicit sequence learning , 2006, Neurobiology of Aging.

[66]  P. Reuter-Lorenz,et al.  Neurocognitive Aging and the Compensation Hypothesis , 2008 .

[67]  C. Lang,et al.  Using Dual Tasks to Test Immediate Transfer of Training Between Naturalistic Movements: A Proof-of-Principle Study , 2012, Journal of motor behavior.

[68]  Faith M. Gunning-Dixon,et al.  Differential aging of the human striatum: a prospective MR imaging study. , 1998, AJNR. American journal of neuroradiology.

[69]  Daphna Shohamy,et al.  Cooperation between the Hippocampus and the Striatum during Episodic Encoding , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[70]  C. Büchel,et al.  The Role of Medial Temporal Lobe Structures in Implicit Learning An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2002, Neuron.

[71]  D. Head,et al.  Frontal-hippocampal double dissociation between normal aging and Alzheimer's disease. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.