The striatum and probabilistic implicit sequence learning

[1]  M. Carpenter,et al.  Functional relationships between the red nucleus and the brachium conjunctivum; physiologic study of lesions of the red nucleus in monkeys with degenerated superior cerebellar brachia. , 1957, Neurology.

[2]  M. Hoehn,et al.  Parkinsonism , 1967, Neurology.

[3]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[4]  G. Schwartz,et al.  Consciousness and Self-Regulation , 1976 .

[5]  L. Squire,et al.  Preserved learning and retention of pattern-analyzing skill in amnesia: dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. , 1980, Science.

[6]  L. Cermak,et al.  Memory disorders associated with huntington's disease: Verbal recall, verbal recognition and procedural memory , 1985, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  M. Nissen,et al.  Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[8]  C. Marsden,et al.  'Frontal' cognitive function in patients with Parkinson's disease 'on' and 'off' levodopa. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[9]  M. Packard,et al.  Differential effects of fornix and caudate nucleus lesions on two radial maze tasks: evidence for multiple memory systems , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[10]  L. Jacoby,et al.  Becoming famous without being recognized: Unconscious influences of memory produced by dividing attention , 1989 .

[11]  L. Jacoby A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory , 1991 .

[12]  C. Marsden,et al.  Dual task performance and processing resources in normal subjects and patients with Parkinson's disease. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[13]  W. Poewe,et al.  High‐speed memory scanning in Parkmson's disease: Adverse effects of levodopa , 1991, Annals of neurology.

[14]  M. Nissen,et al.  Procedural learning is impaired in Huntington's disease: Evidence from the serial reaction time task , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  J. D. McGaugh,et al.  Double dissociation of fornix and caudate nucleus lesions on acquisition of two water maze tasks: further evidence for multiple memory systems. , 1992, Behavioral neuroscience.

[16]  P. Merikle,et al.  Perception without awareness. Critical issues. , 1992, The American psychologist.

[17]  J. Hughes,et al.  Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson's disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases. , 1992, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[18]  M. Amorim,et al.  Conscious knowledge and changes in performance in sequence learning: evidence against dissociation. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[19]  D. Willingham,et al.  Evidence for dissociable motor skills in Huntington’s disease patients , 1993, Psychobiology.

[20]  C D Marsden,et al.  The execution of bimanual movements in patients with Parkinson's, Huntington's and cerebellar disease. , 1993, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[21]  D. Balota,et al.  Implicit Memory and the Formation of New Associations in Nondemented Parkinson′s Disease Individuals and Individuals with Senile Dementia of the Alzheimer Type: A Serial Reaction Time (SRT) Investigation , 1993, Brain and Cognition.

[22]  M. Hallett,et al.  Procedural learning in Parkinson's disease and cerebellar degeneration , 1993, Annals of neurology.

[23]  Daniel B. Willingham,et al.  Dissociation in a serial response time task using a recognition measure: comment on Perruchet and Amorim (1992) , 1993 .

[24]  Andrew P. Yonelinas,et al.  Separating conscious and unconscious influences of memory: measuring recollection , 1993 .

[25]  D. Shanks,et al.  Characteristics of dissociable human learning systems , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[26]  D. Brooks,et al.  Motor sequence learning: a study with positron emission tomography , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[27]  L. Squire,et al.  Parallel brain systems for learning with and without awareness. , 1994, Learning & memory.

[28]  Peder J. Johnson,et al.  Assessing implicit learning with indirect tests: Determining what is learned about sequence structure. , 1994 .

[29]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  A PET investigation of implicit and explicit sequence learning , 1995 .

[30]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Functional Mapping of Sequence Learning in Normal Humans , 1995, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[31]  L. Henderson,et al.  Serial reaction time learning and Parkinson's disease: Evidence for a procedural learning deficit , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[32]  Jane S. Paulsen,et al.  Dissociations within nondeclarative memory in Huntington's disease. , 1996 .

[33]  Jennifer A. Mangels,et al.  A Neostriatal Habit Learning System in Humans , 1996, Science.

[34]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Functional Anatomy of Visuomotor Skill Learning in Human Subjects Examined with Positron Emission Tomography , 1996, The European journal of neuroscience.

[35]  J. Doyon,et al.  Role of the Striatum, Cerebellum, and Frontal Lobes in the Learning of a Visuomotor Sequence , 1997, Brain and Cognition.

[36]  Tim Curran,et al.  Effects of aging on implicit sequence learning: Accounting for sequence structure and explicit knowledge , 1997, Psychological research.

[37]  S. Rauch,et al.  Striatal recruitment during an implicit sequence learning task as measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging , 1997, Human brain mapping.

[38]  C. Marsden,et al.  Learning manual pursuit tracking skills in patients with Parkinson's disease. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[39]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Attention and stimulus characteristics determine the locus of motor-sequence encoding. A PET study. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[40]  Michael A. Stadler,et al.  Handbook of implicit learning , 1998 .

[41]  R. Siegert,et al.  Implicit learning in Parkinson's disease: evidence from a verbal version of the serial reaction time task. , 1998, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[42]  J. Doyon,et al.  Role of the striatum, cerebellum and frontal lobes in the automatization of a repeated visuomotor sequence of movements , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  W. Schultz Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  M. Hallett,et al.  Dynamic cortical involvement in implicit and explicit motor sequence learning. A PET study. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[45]  L. Squire,et al.  Encapsulation of Implicit and Explicit Memory in Sequence Learning , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[46]  Shanks,et al.  Implicit knowledge in sequential learning tasks , 1998 .

[47]  J. Houk,et al.  Model of cortical-basal ganglionic processing: encoding the serial order of sensory events. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  D R Shanks,et al.  Evaluating the relationship between explicit and implicit knowledge in a sequential reaction time task. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[49]  Mark Hallett,et al.  Learning in Parkinson’s disease: eyeblink conditioning, declarative learning, and procedural learning , 1999, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[50]  Carol A. Seger,et al.  Striatal activation during acquisition of a cognitive skill. , 1999, Neuropsychology.

[51]  C. I. Connolly,et al.  Building neural representations of habits. , 1999, Science.

[52]  Andy P. Field,et al.  Discovering Statistics Using SPSS , 2000 .

[53]  J. Hollerman,et al.  Reward processing in primate orbitofrontal cortex and basal ganglia. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[54]  W. Schultz Multiple reward signals in the brain , 2000, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[55]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  Striatum forever, despite sequence learning variability: A random effect analysis of PET data , 2000, Human brain mapping.

[56]  Irene Daum,et al.  Sequence learning in Parkinson’s disease: a comparison of spatial-attention and number-response sequences , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[57]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Visual habit formation in monkeys with neurotoxic lesions of the ventrocaudal neostriatum , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[58]  John McDowall,et al.  Preserved Implicit Learning on Both the Serial Reaction Time Task and Artificial Grammar in Patients with Parkinson's Disease , 2001, Brain and Cognition.

[59]  M. Gluck,et al.  Interactive memory systems in the human brain , 2001, Nature.

[60]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  Can sequence learning be implicit? New evidence with the process dissociation procedure , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[61]  S Channon,et al.  Dissociation between intentional and incidental sequence learning in Huntington's disease. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[62]  D. Brooks Functional imaging studies on dopamine and motor control , 2001, Journal of Neural Transmission.

[63]  T. Robbins,et al.  Enhanced or impaired cognitive function in Parkinson's disease as a function of dopaminergic medication and task demands. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[64]  J. Eastwood,et al.  Perception without awareness: perspectives from cognitive psychology , 2001, Cognition.

[65]  K. Witt,et al.  Dissociation of Habit-Learning in Parkinson's and Cerebellar Disease , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[66]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  Direct comparison of neural systems mediating conscious and unconscious skill learning. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[67]  D. Shanks,et al.  Dissociation between priming and recognition in the expression of sequential knowledge , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[68]  D. Shanks,et al.  Effects of a secondary task on "implicit" sequence learning: learning or performance? , 2002, Psychological research.

[69]  Barbara J Knowlton,et al.  Analysis of probabilistic classification learning in patients with Parkinson's disease before and after pallidotomy surgery. , 2003, Learning & memory.

[70]  Attention and implicit learning , 2003 .

[71]  T. Robbins,et al.  l-Dopa medication remediates cognitive inflexibility, but increases impulsivity in patients with Parkinson’s disease , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[72]  Shelley Channon,et al.  Relationship between priming and recognition in deterministic and probabilistic sequence learning. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[73]  Luis Jiménez,et al.  Temporal effects in sequence learning , 2003 .

[74]  M. Ziessler,et al.  Sequence learning in Parkinson’s disease: The effect of spatial stimulus–response compatibility , 2003, Brain and Cognition.

[75]  Marjan Jahanshahi,et al.  Pallidotomy and incidental sequence learning in Parkinson's disease , 2003, Neuroreport.

[76]  C. Rivat,et al.  Opioid-induced hyperalgesia: abnormal or normal pain? , 2003, Neuroreport.

[77]  Claude Ghez,et al.  Learning networks in health and Parkinson's disease: Reproducibility and treatment effects , 2003, Human brain mapping.

[78]  Richard B. Ivry,et al.  Spatial and Temporal Sequence Learning in Patients with Parkinson's Disease or Cerebellar Lesions , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[79]  M. Gluck,et al.  Human midbrain sensitivity to cognitive feedback and uncertainty during classification learning. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[80]  John C Rothwell,et al.  The effect of short-duration bursts of high-frequency, low-intensity transcranial magnetic stimulation on the human motor cortex , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[81]  M. Petrides,et al.  Neural Bases of Set-Shifting Deficits in Parkinson's Disease , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[82]  Marjan Jahanshahi,et al.  Learning of ambiguous versus hybrid sequences by patients with Parkinson’s disease , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[83]  M. Gluck,et al.  Cortico-striatal contributions to feedback-based learning: converging data from neuroimaging and neuropsychology. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[84]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  By Carrot or by Stick: Cognitive Reinforcement Learning in Parkinsonism , 2004, Science.

[85]  Leonora Wilkinson,et al.  Intentional control and implicit sequence learning. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[86]  M. Gluck,et al.  Role of the basal ganglia in category learning: how do patients with Parkinson's disease learn? , 2004, Behavioral neuroscience.

[87]  C. Marsden,et al.  l-Dopa withdrawal in Parkinson's disease selectively impairs cognitive performance in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction , 2005, Psychopharmacology.

[88]  M. Gluck,et al.  The role of dopamine in cognitive sequence learning: evidence from Parkinson’s disease , 2005, Behavioural Brain Research.

[89]  Carol A. Seger,et al.  The Roles of the Caudate Nucleus in Human Classification Learning , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[90]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Dynamic Dopamine Modulation in the Basal Ganglia: A Neurocomputational Account of Cognitive Deficits in Medicated and Nonmedicated Parkinsonism , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[91]  Russell A. Poldrack,et al.  Long-term test–retest reliability of functional MRI in a classification learning task , 2006, NeuroImage.

[92]  M. Weatherall,et al.  Is implicit sequence learning impaired in Parkinson's disease? A meta-analysis. , 2006, Neuropsychology.

[93]  R. Dolan,et al.  Dopamine-dependent prediction errors underpin reward-seeking behaviour in humans , 2006, Nature.

[94]  Shelley Channon,et al.  Disruption of Sequential Priming in Organic and Pharmacological Amnesia: A Role for the Medial Temporal Lobes in Implicit Contextual Learning , 2006, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[95]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Reversal learning in Parkinson's disease depends on medication status and outcome valence , 2006, Neuropsychologia.