Neuronal correlates of theory of mind and empathy: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study in a nonverbal task

Theory of Mind (ToM), the ability to attribute mental states to others, and empathy, the ability to infer emotional experiences, are important processes in social cognition. Brain imaging studies in healthy subjects have described a brain system involving medial prefrontal cortex, superior temporal sulcus and temporal pole in ToM processing. Studies investigating networks associated with empathic responding also suggest involvement of temporal and frontal lobe regions. In this fMRI study, we used a cartoon task derived from Sarfati et al. (1997) [Sarfati, Y., Hardy-Bayle, M.C., Besche, C., Widlocher, D. 1997. Attribution of intentions to others in people with schizophrenia: a non-verbal exploration with comic strips. Schizophrenia Research 25, 199-209.]with both ToM and empathy stimuli in order to allow comparison of brain activations in these two processes. Results of 13 right-handed, healthy, male volunteers were included. Functional images were acquired using a 1.5 T Phillips Gyroscan. Our results confirmed that ToM and empathy stimuli are associated with overlapping but distinct neuronal networks. Common areas of activation included the medial prefrontal cortex, temporoparietal junction and temporal poles. Compared to the empathy condition, ToM stimuli revealed increased activations in lateral orbitofrontal cortex, middle frontal gyrus, cuneus and superior temporal gyrus. Empathy, on the other hand, was associated with enhanced activations of paracingulate, anterior and posterior cingulate and amygdala. We therefore suggest that ToM and empathy both rely on networks associated with making inferences about mental states of others. However, empathic responding requires the additional recruitment of networks involved in emotional processing. These results have implications for our understanding of disorders characterized by impairments of social cognition, such as autism and psychopathy.

[1]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[2]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[3]  G. Fink,et al.  Neural activation during selective attention to subjective emotional responses , 1997, Neuroreport.

[4]  T. Eddy,et al.  What is a pet? , 2003 .

[5]  N. Sadato,et al.  Attention to emotion modulates fMRI activity in human right superior temporal sulcus. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[6]  G. Fink,et al.  Cerebral Representation of One’s Own Past: Neural Networks Involved in Autobiographical Memory , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  The detection of contingency and animacy from simple animations in the human brain. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[8]  J. Blair,et al.  Theory of Mind in the psychopath , 1996 .

[9]  J. Decety,et al.  Brain Regions Involved in the Perception of Gaze: A PET Study , 1998, NeuroImage.

[10]  C. Frith,et al.  Movement and Mind: A Functional Imaging Study of Perception and Interpretation of Complex Intentional Movement Patterns , 2000, NeuroImage.

[11]  Otto Muzik,et al.  Consistency analysis for digitized 3D Talairach's coplanar stereotactic atlas of the human brain , 1997, Photonics West - Biomedical Optics.

[12]  C. N. Macrae,et al.  Finding the Self? An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  R. Blair,et al.  Neurobiological basis of psychopathy. , 2003, The British journal of psychiatry : the journal of mental science.

[14]  M. Buonocore,et al.  Posterior cingulate cortex activation by emotional words: fMRI evidence from a valence decision task , 2003, Human brain mapping.

[15]  V. Gallese The Roots of Empathy: The Shared Manifold Hypothesis and the Neural Basis of Intersubjectivity , 2003, Psychopathology.

[16]  G. Dunbar,et al.  The Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI). A short diagnostic structured interview: reliability and validity according to the CIDI , 1997, European Psychiatry.

[17]  Y. Sarfati,et al.  Attribution of intentions to others in people with schizophrenia: a non-verbal exploration with comic strips , 1997, Schizophrenia Research.

[18]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Recognition of Mental State Terms , 1994, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[19]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Distinct neural systems subserve person and object knowledge , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[20]  Christopher D. Frith,et al.  Imaging the Intentional Stance in a Competitive Game , 2002, NeuroImage.

[21]  D. Perrett,et al.  Imitation, mirror neurons and autism , 2001, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[22]  U. Frith Mind Blindness and the Brain in Autism , 2001, Neuron.

[23]  P. A. Jacobs,et al.  Evidence from Turner's syndrome of an imprinted X-linked locus affecting cognitive function , 1997, Nature.

[24]  Daniel Houser,et al.  A functional imaging study of cooperation in two-person reciprocal exchange , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[25]  R. Dolan,et al.  An fMRI study of intentional and unintentional (embarrassing) violations of social norms. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[26]  Jane F. Banfield,et al.  Medial prefrontal activity predicts memory for self. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[27]  Evelyn C. Ferstl,et al.  What Does the Frontomedian Cortex Contribute to Language Processing: Coherence or Theory of Mind? , 2002, NeuroImage.

[28]  N. Sadato,et al.  Brain regions involved in verbal or non‐verbal aspects of facial emotion recognition , 2000, Neuroreport.

[29]  H. Soderstrom,et al.  Psychopathy as a disorder of empathy , 2003, European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry.

[30]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: A relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[31]  E. Maguire,et al.  The functional neuroanatomy of comprehension and memory: the importance of prior knowledge. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[32]  Uta Frith,et al.  The Biological Basis of Social Interaction , 2001 .

[33]  Uta Frith,et al.  Theory of mind , 2001, Current Biology.

[34]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Functional anatomy of a common semantic system for words and pictures , 1996, Nature.

[35]  C. Frith,et al.  Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  J. Decety,et al.  A PET Investigation of the Attribution of Intentions with a Nonverbal Task , 2000, NeuroImage.

[37]  David Silbersweig,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy of verbal self-monitoring , 1996 .

[38]  Andrew Calder,et al.  Acquired theory of mind impairments in individuals with bilateral amygdala lesions , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  D. Stuss,et al.  The frontal lobes are necessary for 'theory of mind'. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[40]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Neuroimaging studies of working memory: , 2003, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[41]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension , 1995, Cognition.

[42]  Daniel R. Weinberger,et al.  Changing patterns of brain activation during maze learning , 1998, Brain Research.

[43]  H. Brownell,et al.  Acquired `theory of mind' impairments following stroke , 1999, Cognition.

[44]  I. Wilkinson,et al.  Investigating the functional anatomy of empathy and forgiveness , 2001, Neuroreport.

[45]  Ivanei E. Bramati,et al.  The Neural Correlates of Moral Sensitivity: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Investigation of Basic and Moral Emotions , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[46]  B M Bly,et al.  Functional anatomy of object recognition in humans: evidence from positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging , 1997, Current opinion in neurology.

[47]  R Saxe,et al.  People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.

[48]  Henrik Walter,et al.  Understanding Intentions in Social Interaction: The Role of the Anterior Paracingulate Cortex , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[49]  T. Allison,et al.  Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[50]  R. Gordon Folk Psychology as Simulation , 1986 .

[51]  D. Premack,et al.  Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? , 1978, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[52]  M J Brammer,et al.  Exploring the social brain in schizophrenia: left prefrontal underactivation during mental state attribution. , 2000, The American journal of psychiatry.

[53]  C. Frith,et al.  Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’ , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[54]  G. Dunbar,et al.  The validity of the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) according to the SCID-P and its reliability , 1997, European Psychiatry.

[55]  C. Frith,et al.  Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[56]  R. Adolphs Cognitive neuroscience: Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[57]  K. Zilles,et al.  Mind Reading: Neural Mechanisms of Theory of Mind and Self-Perspective , 2001, NeuroImage.

[58]  A. Gopnik,et al.  Children's understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance-reality distinction. , 1988, Child development.

[59]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Empathy for Pain Involves the Affective but not Sensory Components of Pain , 2004, Science.

[60]  C. Frith,et al.  Interacting minds--a biological basis. , 1999, Science.

[61]  E T Rolls,et al.  Impulsivity, time perception, emotion and reinforcement sensitivity in patients with orbitofrontal cortex lesions. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[62]  M. Hallett,et al.  Modeling other minds , 1995, Neuroreport.

[63]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Central cancellation of self-produced tickle sensation , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[64]  J. Decety,et al.  How Would You Feel versus How Do You Think She Would Feel? A Neuroimaging Study of Perspective-Taking with Social Emotions , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[65]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: an fMRI study , 1999, The European journal of neuroscience.