Four- and 5-Year-Olds Infer Differences in Relative Ability and Appropriately Allocate Roles to Achieve Cooperative, Competitive, and Prosocial Goals
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] V. Jaswal,et al. Characterizing children's expectations about expertise and incompetence: halo or pitchfork effects? , 2011, Child development.
[2] M. Tomasello,et al. The roots of human altruism. , 2009, British journal of psychology.
[3] J. Hamlin,et al. Moral Judgment and Action in Preverbal Infants and Toddlers , 2013 .
[4] Geetha B. Ramani,et al. Becoming a social partner with peers: cooperation and social understanding in one- and two-year-olds. , 2006, Child development.
[5] H. Ross,et al. Communication within infant social games. , 1987 .
[6] Dale F. Hay,et al. Cooperative interactions and sharing between very young children and their parents. , 1979 .
[7] Amanda L. Woodward,et al. Infants track action goals within and across agents , 2007, Cognition.
[8] M Taylor,et al. Perspective taking and theory of mind: do children predict interpretive diversity as a function of differences in observers' knowledge? , 1991, Child development.
[9] Gail D. Heyman,et al. Young children's vulnerability to self-blame and helplessness: relationship to beliefs about goodness. , 1992, Child development.
[10] Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al. Foundations of cooperation in young children , 2008, Cognition.
[11] Joshua B. Tenenbaum,et al. Children’s understanding of the costs and rewards underlying rational action , 2015, Cognition.
[12] M. Tomasello,et al. Helping and Cooperation at 14 Months of Age. , 2007, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[13] Ulf Liszkowski,et al. 18-Month-Olds Predict Specific Action Mistakes Through Attribution of False Belief, Not Ignorance, and Intervene Accordingly. , 2012, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[14] Peter DeScioli,et al. Fairness versus favoritism in children , 2012 .
[15] Michael Tomasello,et al. Cooperative Problem‐Solving and Teaching in Preschoolers , 2001 .
[16] Samuel Ronfard,et al. Cognitive Mechanisms Associated with Children’s Selective Teaching , 2018 .
[17] M. Tomasello,et al. Children's developing understanding of legitimate reasons for allocating resources unequally , 2016 .
[18] F. Warneken,et al. Precocious Prosociality: Why Do Young Children Help? , 2015 .
[19] L. Schulz,et al. From Exploration to Instruction: Children Learn From Exploration and Tailor Their Demonstrations to Observers' Goals and Competence. , 2019, Child development.
[20] Andrei Cimpian,et al. Who Is Good at This Game? Linking an Activity to a Social Category Undermines Children’s Achievement , 2012, Psychological science.
[21] Alison Gopnik,et al. I know what you need to know: Children's developing theory of mind and pedagogical evidence selection , 2017, CogSci.
[22] C. Chevallier,et al. Preschoolers are able to take merit into account when distributing goods. , 2012, Developmental psychology.
[23] B. Rockenbach,et al. Egalitarianism in young children , 2008, Nature.
[24] Samuel Ronfard,et al. Children teach methods they could not discover for themselves. , 2016, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[25] M. Tomasello,et al. Eighteen-month-old infants show false belief understanding in an active helping paradigm , 2009, Cognition.
[26] F. Warneken,et al. How Children Solve the Two Challenges of Cooperation , 2018, Annual review of psychology.
[27] M. Tomasello,et al. Differences in Cognitive Processes Underlying the Collaborative Activities of Children and Chimpanzees. , 2012 .
[28] Joshua B. Tenenbaum,et al. The Naïve Utility Calculus: Computational Principles Underlying Commonsense Psychology , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[29] J. Hamlin,et al. Social evaluation by preverbal infants , 2007, Nature.
[30] M. Tomasello,et al. 12- and 18-Month-Olds Point to Provide Information for Others , 2006 .
[31] Frank C Keil,et al. Early understanding of the division of cognitive labor. , 2002, Child development.
[32] Joshua B. Tenenbaum,et al. Ten-month-old infants infer the value of goals from the costs of actions , 2017, Science.
[33] M. Tomasello,et al. Young children enforce social norms selectively depending on the violator’s group affiliation , 2012, Cognition.
[34] C. Legare,et al. Instrumental and Conventional Interpretations of Behavior Are Associated With Distinct Outcomes in Early Childhood. , 2016, Child development.
[35] P. Harris,et al. I Should but I Won’t: Why Young Children Endorse Norms of Fair Sharing but Do Not Follow Them , 2013, PloS one.
[36] Ruth Rechis,et al. Children's cooperative and competitive interactions in limited resource situations: A literature review , 2006 .
[37] C. Boesch,et al. Hunting behavior of wild chimpanzees in the Taï National Park. , 1989, American journal of physical anthropology.
[38] Julian Jara-Ettinger,et al. Children consider others' expected costs and rewards when deciding what to teach , 2016, CogSci.
[39] M. Tomasello,et al. Twelve-month-olds communicate helpfully and appropriately for knowledgeable and ignorant partners , 2008, Cognition.
[40] Mariano Sigman,et al. Teaching, naturally , 2014, Trends in Neuroscience and Education.
[41] M. Tomasello,et al. Unwilling versus unable: infants' understanding of intentional action. , 2005, Developmental psychology.
[42] W. Schneider. Performance prediction in young children: Effects of skill, metacognition and wishful thinking , 1998 .
[43] Cara DiYanni,et al. Looking good versus doing good: which factors take precedence when children learn about new tools? , 2011, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[44] M. Tomasello,et al. Understanding "prior intentions" enables two-year-olds to imitatively learn a complex task. , 2002, Child development.
[45] Julia W. Van de Vondervoort,et al. Evidence for Intuitive Morality: Preverbal Infants Make Sociomoral Evaluations , 2016 .
[46] P. Harris,et al. Trust in Testimony: Children's Use of True and False Statements , 2004, Psychological science.
[47] T. Kushnir,et al. "Who can help me fix this toy?" The distinction between causal knowledge and word knowledge guides preschoolers' selective requests for information. , 2013, Developmental psychology.
[48] Frank C Keil,et al. Should you ask a fisherman or a biologist?: Developmental shifts in ways of clustering knowledge. , 2004, Child development.
[49] M. Tomasello,et al. Altruistic Helping in Human Infants and Young Chimpanzees , 2006, Science.
[50] Jessica A. Sommerville,et al. Fairness Expectations and Altruistic Sharing in 15-Month-Old Human Infants , 2011, PloS one.
[51] P. Blake,et al. “I had so much it didn’t seem fair”: Eight-year-olds reject two forms of inequity , 2011, Cognition.
[52] M. Tomasello,et al. Procedural justice in children: Preschoolers accept unequal resource distributions if the procedure provides equal opportunities. , 2015, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[53] A. Meltzoff. Understanding the Intentions of Others: Re-Enactment of Intended Acts by 18-Month-Old Children. , 1995, Developmental psychology.
[54] Celia A. Brownell,et al. Changes in cooperation and self-other differentiation during the second year. , 1990, Child development.
[55] R. Butler,et al. Age trends in the use of social and temporal comparison for self-evaluation: examination of a novel developmental hypothesis. , 1998, Child development.
[56] Celia A. Brownell,et al. To share or not to share: When do toddlers respond to another's needs? , 2009, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[57] K. Olson,et al. When kids know better: paternalistic helping in 3-year-old children. , 2013, Developmental psychology.
[58] C. Dweck,et al. Helplessness in early childhood: the role of contingent worth. , 1995, Child development.
[59] Daniel Brickman,et al. Preschoolers' Responses to Social Comparisons Involving Relative Failure , 2008, Psychological science.
[60] Grace Bennett-Pierre,et al. Reverse-engineering the process: Adults' and preschoolers' ability to infer the difficulty of novel tasks , 2017, CogSci.
[61] M. Tomasello,et al. Young children's planning in a collaborative problem-solving task , 2014 .
[62] J. Tenenbaum,et al. Not So Innocent , 2015, Psychological science.
[63] V. Jaswal,et al. Adults Don't Always Know Best , 2006, Psychological science.
[64] M. Vanderborght,et al. Who Knows Best? Preschoolers Sometimes Prefer Child Informants Over Adult Informants. , 2009, Infant and child development.
[65] D. Frye,et al. Children's understanding of teaching: the role of knowledge and belief , 2004 .
[66] M. Tomasello,et al. Cooperative activities in young children and chimpanzees. , 2006, Child development.
[67] M. Tomasello. The Human Adaptation for Culture , 1999 .
[68] A. Shaw,et al. Children discard a resource to avoid inequity. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[69] Laura Schulz,et al. Quit while you're ahead: Preschoolers' persistence and willingness to accept challenges are affected by social comparison , 2015, CogSci.