Four- and 5-Year-Olds Infer Differences in Relative Ability and Appropriately Allocate Roles to Achieve Cooperative, Competitive, and Prosocial Goals

Preschoolers are sensitive to differences in individuals’ access to external resources (e.g., tools) in division of labor tasks. However, little is known about whether children consider differences in individuals’ internal resources (e.g., abilities) and whether children can flexibly allocate roles across different goal contexts. Critically, factors that are relevant to role allocation in collaborative contexts may be irrelevant in competitive and prosocial ones. In three preregistered experiments, we found that 4- and 5-year-olds (mean: 54 months; range: 42–66 months; N = 132) used age differences to infer relative ability and appropriately allocate the harder and easier of two tasks in a dyadic cooperative interaction (Experiment 1), and appropriately ignored relative ability in competitive (Experiment 2) and prosocial (Experiment 3) contexts, instead assigning others the harder and easier roles, respectively. Thus, 3-and-a-half- to 5-year-olds evaluate their own abilities relative to others and effectively allocate roles to achieve diverse goals.

[1]  V. Jaswal,et al.  Characterizing children's expectations about expertise and incompetence: halo or pitchfork effects? , 2011, Child development.

[2]  M. Tomasello,et al.  The roots of human altruism. , 2009, British journal of psychology.

[3]  J. Hamlin,et al.  Moral Judgment and Action in Preverbal Infants and Toddlers , 2013 .

[4]  Geetha B. Ramani,et al.  Becoming a social partner with peers: cooperation and social understanding in one- and two-year-olds. , 2006, Child development.

[5]  H. Ross,et al.  Communication within infant social games. , 1987 .

[6]  Dale F. Hay,et al.  Cooperative interactions and sharing between very young children and their parents. , 1979 .

[7]  Amanda L. Woodward,et al.  Infants track action goals within and across agents , 2007, Cognition.

[8]  M Taylor,et al.  Perspective taking and theory of mind: do children predict interpretive diversity as a function of differences in observers' knowledge? , 1991, Child development.

[9]  Gail D. Heyman,et al.  Young children's vulnerability to self-blame and helplessness: relationship to beliefs about goodness. , 1992, Child development.

[10]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  Foundations of cooperation in young children , 2008, Cognition.

[11]  Joshua B. Tenenbaum,et al.  Children’s understanding of the costs and rewards underlying rational action , 2015, Cognition.

[12]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Helping and Cooperation at 14 Months of Age. , 2007, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[13]  Ulf Liszkowski,et al.  18-Month-Olds Predict Specific Action Mistakes Through Attribution of False Belief, Not Ignorance, and Intervene Accordingly. , 2012, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[14]  Peter DeScioli,et al.  Fairness versus favoritism in children , 2012 .

[15]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Cooperative Problem‐Solving and Teaching in Preschoolers , 2001 .

[16]  Samuel Ronfard,et al.  Cognitive Mechanisms Associated with Children’s Selective Teaching , 2018 .

[17]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Children's developing understanding of legitimate reasons for allocating resources unequally , 2016 .

[18]  F. Warneken,et al.  Precocious Prosociality: Why Do Young Children Help? , 2015 .

[19]  L. Schulz,et al.  From Exploration to Instruction: Children Learn From Exploration and Tailor Their Demonstrations to Observers' Goals and Competence. , 2019, Child development.

[20]  Andrei Cimpian,et al.  Who Is Good at This Game? Linking an Activity to a Social Category Undermines Children’s Achievement , 2012, Psychological science.

[21]  Alison Gopnik,et al.  I know what you need to know: Children's developing theory of mind and pedagogical evidence selection , 2017, CogSci.

[22]  C. Chevallier,et al.  Preschoolers are able to take merit into account when distributing goods. , 2012, Developmental psychology.

[23]  B. Rockenbach,et al.  Egalitarianism in young children , 2008, Nature.

[24]  Samuel Ronfard,et al.  Children teach methods they could not discover for themselves. , 2016, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[25]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Eighteen-month-old infants show false belief understanding in an active helping paradigm , 2009, Cognition.

[26]  F. Warneken,et al.  How Children Solve the Two Challenges of Cooperation , 2018, Annual review of psychology.

[27]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Differences in Cognitive Processes Underlying the Collaborative Activities of Children and Chimpanzees. , 2012 .

[28]  Joshua B. Tenenbaum,et al.  The Naïve Utility Calculus: Computational Principles Underlying Commonsense Psychology , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[29]  J. Hamlin,et al.  Social evaluation by preverbal infants , 2007, Nature.

[30]  M. Tomasello,et al.  12- and 18-Month-Olds Point to Provide Information for Others , 2006 .

[31]  Frank C Keil,et al.  Early understanding of the division of cognitive labor. , 2002, Child development.

[32]  Joshua B. Tenenbaum,et al.  Ten-month-old infants infer the value of goals from the costs of actions , 2017, Science.

[33]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Young children enforce social norms selectively depending on the violator’s group affiliation , 2012, Cognition.

[34]  C. Legare,et al.  Instrumental and Conventional Interpretations of Behavior Are Associated With Distinct Outcomes in Early Childhood. , 2016, Child development.

[35]  P. Harris,et al.  I Should but I Won’t: Why Young Children Endorse Norms of Fair Sharing but Do Not Follow Them , 2013, PloS one.

[36]  Ruth Rechis,et al.  Children's cooperative and competitive interactions in limited resource situations: A literature review , 2006 .

[37]  C. Boesch,et al.  Hunting behavior of wild chimpanzees in the Taï National Park. , 1989, American journal of physical anthropology.

[38]  Julian Jara-Ettinger,et al.  Children consider others' expected costs and rewards when deciding what to teach , 2016, CogSci.

[39]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Twelve-month-olds communicate helpfully and appropriately for knowledgeable and ignorant partners , 2008, Cognition.

[40]  Mariano Sigman,et al.  Teaching, naturally , 2014, Trends in Neuroscience and Education.

[41]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Unwilling versus unable: infants' understanding of intentional action. , 2005, Developmental psychology.

[42]  W. Schneider Performance prediction in young children: Effects of skill, metacognition and wishful thinking , 1998 .

[43]  Cara DiYanni,et al.  Looking good versus doing good: which factors take precedence when children learn about new tools? , 2011, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[44]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Understanding "prior intentions" enables two-year-olds to imitatively learn a complex task. , 2002, Child development.

[45]  Julia W. Van de Vondervoort,et al.  Evidence for Intuitive Morality: Preverbal Infants Make Sociomoral Evaluations , 2016 .

[46]  P. Harris,et al.  Trust in Testimony: Children's Use of True and False Statements , 2004, Psychological science.

[47]  T. Kushnir,et al.  "Who can help me fix this toy?" The distinction between causal knowledge and word knowledge guides preschoolers' selective requests for information. , 2013, Developmental psychology.

[48]  Frank C Keil,et al.  Should you ask a fisherman or a biologist?: Developmental shifts in ways of clustering knowledge. , 2004, Child development.

[49]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Altruistic Helping in Human Infants and Young Chimpanzees , 2006, Science.

[50]  Jessica A. Sommerville,et al.  Fairness Expectations and Altruistic Sharing in 15-Month-Old Human Infants , 2011, PloS one.

[51]  P. Blake,et al.  “I had so much it didn’t seem fair”: Eight-year-olds reject two forms of inequity , 2011, Cognition.

[52]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Procedural justice in children: Preschoolers accept unequal resource distributions if the procedure provides equal opportunities. , 2015, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[53]  A. Meltzoff Understanding the Intentions of Others: Re-Enactment of Intended Acts by 18-Month-Old Children. , 1995, Developmental psychology.

[54]  Celia A. Brownell,et al.  Changes in cooperation and self-other differentiation during the second year. , 1990, Child development.

[55]  R. Butler,et al.  Age trends in the use of social and temporal comparison for self-evaluation: examination of a novel developmental hypothesis. , 1998, Child development.

[56]  Celia A. Brownell,et al.  To share or not to share: When do toddlers respond to another's needs? , 2009, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[57]  K. Olson,et al.  When kids know better: paternalistic helping in 3-year-old children. , 2013, Developmental psychology.

[58]  C. Dweck,et al.  Helplessness in early childhood: the role of contingent worth. , 1995, Child development.

[59]  Daniel Brickman,et al.  Preschoolers' Responses to Social Comparisons Involving Relative Failure , 2008, Psychological science.

[60]  Grace Bennett-Pierre,et al.  Reverse-engineering the process: Adults' and preschoolers' ability to infer the difficulty of novel tasks , 2017, CogSci.

[61]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Young children's planning in a collaborative problem-solving task , 2014 .

[62]  J. Tenenbaum,et al.  Not So Innocent , 2015, Psychological science.

[63]  V. Jaswal,et al.  Adults Don't Always Know Best , 2006, Psychological science.

[64]  M. Vanderborght,et al.  Who Knows Best? Preschoolers Sometimes Prefer Child Informants Over Adult Informants. , 2009, Infant and child development.

[65]  D. Frye,et al.  Children's understanding of teaching: the role of knowledge and belief , 2004 .

[66]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Cooperative activities in young children and chimpanzees. , 2006, Child development.

[67]  M. Tomasello The Human Adaptation for Culture , 1999 .

[68]  A. Shaw,et al.  Children discard a resource to avoid inequity. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[69]  Laura Schulz,et al.  Quit while you're ahead: Preschoolers' persistence and willingness to accept challenges are affected by social comparison , 2015, CogSci.