Representation, Control, or Reasoning? Distinct Functions for Theory of Mind within the Medial Prefrontal Cortex

The medial pFC (mPFC) is frequently reported to play a central role in Theory of Mind (ToM). However, the contribution of this large cortical region in ToM is not well understood. Combining a novel behavioral task with fMRI, we sought to demonstrate functional divisions between dorsal and rostral mPFC. All conditions of the task required the representation of mental states (beliefs and desires). The level of demands on cognitive control (high vs. low) and the nature of the demands on reasoning (deductive vs. abductive) were varied orthogonally between conditions. Activation in dorsal mPFC was modulated by the need for control, whereas rostral mPFC was modulated by reasoning demands. These findings fit with previously suggested domain-general functions for different parts of mPFC and suggest that these functions are recruited selectively in the service of ToM.

[1]  R. Mar The neural bases of social cognition and story comprehension. , 2011, Annual review of psychology.

[2]  Lindsey J. Powell,et al.  It's the Thought That Counts , 2006, Psychological science.

[3]  C. Frith,et al.  Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[4]  F. Van Overwalle Social cognition and the brain: A meta‐analysis , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[5]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Self-perspective inhibition deficits cannot be explained by general executive control difficulties , 2015, Cortex.

[6]  Beate Sodian,et al.  Functional activity of the right temporo-parietal junction and of the medial prefrontal cortex associated with true and false belief reasoning , 2012, NeuroImage.

[7]  R. Saxe,et al.  Making sense of another mind: The role of the right temporo-parietal junction , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  Tim Menzies,et al.  Applications of abduction: knowledge-level modelling , 1996, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[9]  J. A. Fodor A theory of the child's theory of mind , 1992, Cognition.

[10]  Beate Sodian,et al.  Common and distinct neural networks for false-belief reasoning and inhibitory control , 2011, NeuroImage.

[11]  Jessica A. Hehman,et al.  Representational and executive selection resources in ‘theory of mind’: Evidence from compromised belief-desire reasoning in old age , 2006, Cognition.

[12]  J. Bargh,et al.  Automaticity of social behavior: direct effects of trait construct and stereotype-activation on action. , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[13]  R Saxe,et al.  People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.

[14]  C. Frith,et al.  Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[15]  James R. Booth,et al.  The Brain Network for Deductive Reasoning: A Quantitative Meta-analysis of 28 Neuroimaging Studies , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[16]  A. Owen,et al.  Anterior prefrontal cortex: insights into function from anatomy and neuroimaging , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[17]  M. Banaji,et al.  Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. , 1995, Psychological review.

[18]  Beate Sodian,et al.  Neural correlates of true and false belief reasoning , 2007, NeuroImage.

[19]  Josef Perner,et al.  Temporo-parietal Junction Activity in Theory-of-Mind Tasks: Falseness, Beliefs, or Attention , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  I. Toni,et al.  Altered Communicative Decisions following Ventromedial Prefrontal Lesions , 2015, Current Biology.

[21]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Mentalizing under Uncertainty: Dissociated Neural Responses to Ambiguous and Unambiguous Mental State Inferences , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[22]  Andrew D. Engell,et al.  Understanding evaluation of faces on social dimensions , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[23]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[24]  R. Lebow The Future of Memory , 2008 .

[25]  Benjamin O Turner,et al.  Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences Hemispheric Lateralization in Reasoning , 2022 .

[26]  A. Leslie Pretense and representation: The origins of "theory of mind." , 1987 .

[27]  C. Frith,et al.  Distinct regions of medial rostral prefrontal cortex supporting social and nonsocial functions. , 2007, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[28]  Emery N. Brown,et al.  Distinct Regions of Right Temporo-Parietal Junction Are Selective for Theory of Mind and Exogenous Attention , 2009, PloS one.

[29]  Peter C. Hansen,et al.  Multiple roles for executive control in belief–desire reasoning: Distinct neural networks are recruited for self perspective inhibition and complexity of reasoning , 2012, NeuroImage.

[30]  Berthold Langguth,et al.  Inhibiting the posterior medial prefrontal cortex by rTMS decreases the discrepancy between self and other in Theory of Mind reasoning , 2014, Behavioural Brain Research.

[31]  Susanne Quadflieg,et al.  In our own image? Emotional and neural processing differences when observing human–human vs human–robot interactions , 2015, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[32]  Ian A Apperly,et al.  Developmental continuity in theory of mind: speed and accuracy of belief-desire reasoning in children and adults. , 2011, Child development.

[33]  C. Frith,et al.  Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’ , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[34]  C. Frith,et al.  The Neural Basis of Mentalizing , 2006, Neuron.

[35]  Skyler T. Hawk,et al.  Presentation and validation of the Radboud Faces Database , 2010 .

[36]  A. Bailey,et al.  Are there theory of mind regions in the brain? A review of the neuroimaging literature , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[37]  R. Nathan Spreng,et al.  The Common Neural Basis of Autobiographical Memory, Prospection, Navigation, Theory of Mind, and the Default Mode: A Quantitative Meta-analysis , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[38]  Maurice Pagnucco,et al.  The Role of Abductive Reasoning within the Process of Belief Revision , 1996 .

[39]  I. Apperly Mindreaders: The Cognitive Basis of "Theory of Mind" , 2010 .

[40]  Donald W. Pfaff,et al.  Deconstructing and reconstructing theory of mind , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[41]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception , 1983, Cognition.

[42]  S. Shamay-Tsoory,et al.  Neuroanatomical and neurochemical bases of theory of mind , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  Stephen Lawrie,et al.  Functional Specialization within Rostral Prefrontal Cortex (Area 10): A Meta-analysis , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[44]  R. N. Spreng,et al.  The Future of Memory: Remembering, Imagining, and the Brain , 2012, Neuron.

[45]  David J. Turk,et al.  Exploring the Neural Correlates of Social Stereotyping , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[46]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  Social cognitive neuroscience: a review of core processes. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.

[47]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Functional organization of the medial frontal cortex , 2007, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[48]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex , 1999, Nature.

[49]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[50]  M. Posner,et al.  Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[51]  Uta Frith,et al.  Theory of mind , 2001, Current Biology.