The manifold nature of interpersonal relations: the quest for a common mechanism.

It has been proposed that the capacity to code the 'like me' analogy between self and others constitutes a basic prerequisite and a starting point for social cognition. It is by means of this self/other equivalence that meaningful social bonds can be established, that we can recognize others as similar to us, and that imitation can take place. In this article I discuss recent neurophysiological and brain imaging data on monkeys and humans, showing that the 'like me' analogy may rest upon a series of 'mirror-matching' mechanisms. A new conceptual tool able to capture the richness of the experiences we share with others is introduced: the shared manifold of intersubjectivity. I propose that all kinds of interpersonal relations (imitation, empathy and the attribution of intentions) depend, at a basic level, on the constitution of a shared manifold space. This shared space is functionally characterized by automatic, unconscious embodied simulation routines.

[1]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Localization of grasp representations in humans by positron emission tomography , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Localization of grasp representations in humans by PET: 1. Observation versus execution , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  Vittorio Gallese,et al.  A neuroscientific grasp of concepts: from control to representation. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[4]  V. Gallese The Roots of Empathy: The Shared Manifold Hypothesis and the Neural Basis of Intersubjectivity , 2003, Psychopathology.

[5]  R. Byrne Imitation as behaviour parsing. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[6]  C. Frith,et al.  Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[7]  R. Blair,et al.  Facial expressions, their communicatory functions and neuro-cognitive substrates. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[8]  K. Doya,et al.  A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[9]  Stefan Schaal,et al.  http://www.jstor.org/about/terms.html. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained , 2007 .

[10]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Action generation and action perception in imitation: an instance of the ideomotor principle. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[11]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Hearing Sounds, Understanding Actions: Action Representation in Mirror Neurons , 2002, Science.

[12]  A. Meltzoff Elements of a developmental theory of imitation , 2002 .

[13]  Alvin I. Goldman,et al.  The Mentalizing Folk , 2002 .

[14]  E. Visalberghi,et al.  Do monkeys ape?: ten years after , 2002 .

[15]  W. Prinz,et al.  The imitative mind : development, evolution, and brain bases , 2002 .

[16]  J C Mazziotta,et al.  Reafferent copies of imitated actions in the right superior temporal cortex , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[17]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[18]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  I Know What You Are Doing A Neurophysiological Study , 2001, Neuron.

[19]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[20]  Vittorio Gallese,et al.  The eyes, the hand, and the mind: behavioral and neurophysiological aspects of social cognition , 2001 .

[21]  V. Gallese The ‘‘shared manifold’’ hypothesis: from mirror neurons to empathy , 2001 .

[22]  A. Young,et al.  Impaired recognition and experience of disgust following brain injury , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  Daniel J. Povinelli,et al.  Toward a science of other minds: escaping the argument by analogy , 2000, Cogn. Sci..

[24]  Vittorio Gallese,et al.  Reply to Schulkin , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[25]  R. Adolphs,et al.  A Role for Somatosensory Cortices in the Visual Recognition of Emotion as Revealed by Three-Dimensional Lesion Mapping , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  J. Panksepp,et al.  Review: The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness , 2000 .

[27]  V. Gallese Action representaion and the inferior parietal lobule , 2000 .

[28]  Martha J. Farah,et al.  The neural bases of mental imagery , 2000 .

[29]  Daniel C. Richardson,et al.  Eye Movements During Comprehension of Spoken Scene Descriptions , 2000 .

[30]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. , 1999, Science.

[31]  J. Dostrovsky,et al.  Pain-related neurons in the human cingulate cortex , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  J. Decety,et al.  Neural mechanisms subserving the perception of human actions , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[33]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Activation of human primary motor cortex during action observation: a neuromagnetic study. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[34]  A. Goldman,et al.  Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[35]  S. Cochin,et al.  Perception of motion and qEEG activity in human adults. , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[36]  David A. Leavens,et al.  Theory of mind in nonhuman primates , 1998, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[37]  B. Kushner Descartes' error. , 1998, Journal of AAPOS : the official publication of the American Association for Pediatric Ophthalmology and Strabismus.

[38]  V. Jousmäki,et al.  Involvement of Primary Motor Cortex in Motor Imagery: A Neuromagnetic Study , 1997, NeuroImage.

[39]  E. Procyk,et al.  Brain activity during observation of actions. Influence of action content and subject's strategy. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[40]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Explaining Facial Imitation: A Theoretical Model. , 1997, Early development & parenting.

[41]  S. Baron-Cohen Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind , 1997 .

[42]  A. Gopnik,et al.  Words, thoughts, and theories , 1997 .

[43]  R. Byrne,et al.  Machiavellian intelligence II : extensions and evaluations , 1997 .

[44]  L. Stark,et al.  Spontaneous Eye Movements During Visual Imagery Reflect the Content of the Visual Scene , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[45]  M. Diamond,et al.  Primary Motor and Sensory Cortex Activation during Motor Performance and Motor Imagery: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[46]  Andrew Whiten,et al.  Studies of imitation in chimpanzees and children. , 1996 .

[47]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Possible involvement of primary motor cortex in mentally simulated movement: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. , 1996, Neuroreport.

[48]  D. Washburn,et al.  Can monkeys (Macaca mulatta) represent invisible displacement? , 1996, Journal of comparative psychology.

[49]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action recognition in the premotor cortex. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[50]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[51]  Peter K. Smith,et al.  Theories of Theories of Mind , 1996 .

[52]  C. Heyes,et al.  Social learning in animals : the roots of culture , 1996 .

[53]  J. Fodor The elm and the expert : mentalese and its semantics , 1995 .

[54]  A. David Image and Brain: The Resolution of the Imagery Debate . By S. M. Kosslyn. (Pp. 516; £40.50.) MIT Press: Cambridge, Mass. 1994. , 1995, Psychological Medicine.

[55]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[56]  L. Parsons,et al.  Use of implicit motor imagery for visual shape discrimination as revealed by PET , 1995, Nature.

[57]  S. Guttenplan The Elm and the Expert. Mentalese and its Semantics By Jerry A. Fodor MIT Press, 1994, pp. xiv+129, £15.95 , 1995 .

[58]  G. W. Pigman Freud and the history of empathy. , 1995, The International journal of psycho-analysis.

[59]  R. Byrne The Thinking Ape : Evolutionary Origins of Intelligence , 1995 .

[60]  S. Kosslyn Image and Brain: The Resolution of the Imagery Debate , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[61]  M. Jeannerod The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[62]  Steven P. Horst,et al.  The Elm And The Expert , 1994 .

[63]  D Le Bihan,et al.  Activation of human primary visual cortex during visual recall: a magnetic resonance imaging study. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[64]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Visual Mental Imagery Activates Topographically Organized Visual Cortex: PET Investigations , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[65]  A. Goldman The psychology of folk psychology , 1993, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[66]  J. Fodor A theory of the child's theory of mind , 1992, Cognition.

[67]  K. J. Cole,et al.  Strength increases from the motor program: comparison of training with maximal voluntary and imagined muscle contractions. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[68]  A. Goldman In Defense of the Simulation Theory , 1992 .

[69]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Vegetative response during imagined movement is proportional to mental effort , 1991, Behavioural Brain Research.

[70]  J. Decety,et al.  The cerebellum participates in mental activity: tomographic measurements of regional cerebral blood flow , 1990, Brain Research.

[71]  E. Visalberghi,et al.  “Language” and intelligence in monkeys and apes: Do monkeys ape? , 1990 .

[72]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  The timing of mentally represented actions , 1989, Behavioural Brain Research.

[73]  Werner Bohleber,et al.  The Interpersonal World of the Infant , 1989 .

[74]  P. Harris Children and emotion , 1989 .

[75]  Edmund Husserl,et al.  Studies in the phenomenology of constitution , 1989 .

[76]  A. Leslie Pretense and representation: The origins of "theory of mind." , 1987 .

[77]  R. Gordon Folk Psychology as Simulation , 1986 .

[78]  D. Stern The interpersonal world of infant , 1985 .

[79]  R. William,et al.  Ideas Pertaining to a pure Phenomenology and to a Phenomenological Philosophy. First Book: General Introduction to a Pure Phenomenology , 1984 .

[80]  Alvin I. Goldman,et al.  Philosophical Applications Of Cognitive Science , 1983 .

[81]  E. Husserl,et al.  Phenomenology and the Foundations of the sciences. Third book : Ideas pertaining to a pure phenomenology and to a phenomenological Philosophy , 1980 .

[82]  Nicholas Humphrey,et al.  Nature's Psychologists , 1980 .

[83]  P. Roland,et al.  Supplementary motor area and other cortical areas in organization of voluntary movements in man. , 1980, Journal of neurophysiology.

[84]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Intermodal matching by human neonates , 1979, Nature.

[85]  D. Premack,et al.  Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? , 1978, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[86]  S M Kosslyn,et al.  Visual images preserve metric spatial information: evidence from studies of image scanning. , 1978, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[87]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Imitation of Facial and Manual Gestures by Human Neonates , 1977, Science.

[88]  N. Humphrey The Social Function of Intellect , 1976 .