Seeing faces is necessary for face-patch formation

Here we report that monkeys raised without exposure to faces did not develop face domains, but did develop domains for other categories and did show normal retinotopic organization, indicating that early face deprivation leads to a highly selective cortical processing deficit. Therefore, experience must be necessary for the formation (or maintenance) of face domains. Gaze tracking revealed that control monkeys looked preferentially at faces, even at ages prior to the emergence of face domains, but face-deprived monkeys did not, indicating that face looking is not innate. A retinotopic organization is present throughout the visual system at birth, so selective early viewing behavior could bias category-specific visual responses toward particular retinotopic representations, thereby leading to domain formation in stereotyped locations in inferotemporal cortex, without requiring category-specific templates or biases. Thus, we propose that environmental importance influences viewing behavior, viewing behavior drives neuronal activity, and neuronal activity sculpts domain formation.

[1]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Distributed representation of objects in the human ventral visual pathway. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[2]  Talma Hendler,et al.  Eccentricity Bias as an Organizing Principle for Human High-Order Object Areas , 2002, Neuron.

[3]  Jennifer M. D. Yoon,et al.  Human Neuroscience , 2022 .

[4]  Y. Sugita Face perception in monkeys reared with no exposure to faces , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[5]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Scene-Selective Cortical Regions in Human and Nonhuman Primates , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  James W. Tanaka,et al.  The SHINE toolbox for controlling low-level image properties , 2010 .

[7]  O. Pascalis,et al.  Is Face Processing Species-Specific During the First Year of Life? , 2002, Science.

[8]  Xueqi Cheng,et al.  A Network for Scene Processing in the Macaque Temporal Lobe , 2013, Neuron.

[9]  F. Simion,et al.  Can a Nonspecific Bias Toward Top-Heavy Patterns Explain Newborns' Face Preference? , 2004, Psychological science.

[10]  Kristin Andrews,et al.  Evidence and implications , 2011 .

[11]  V. Slaughter,et al.  Comprehensive Longitudinal Study Challenges the Existence of Neonatal Imitation in Humans , 2016, Current Biology.

[12]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  An Area within Human Ventral Cortex Sensitive to “Building” Stimuli Evidence and Implications , 1998, Neuron.

[13]  Patricia S. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Face scanning and responsiveness to social cues in infant rhesus monkeys. , 1982 .

[14]  W. J. Nowack Neurobiology of Neocortex , 1989, Neurology.

[15]  Till S. Hartmann,et al.  End-Stopping Predicts Curvature Tuning along the Ventral Stream , 2017, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  Talma Hendler,et al.  Center–periphery organization of human object areas , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[17]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Cognitive and Neural Development of Face Recognition in Humans , 2009 .

[18]  Daniel D. Dilks,et al.  A critical review of the development of face recognition: Experience is less important than previously believed , 2012, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[19]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Functional imaging of the monkey brain , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[20]  C. C. Goren,et al.  Visual following and pattern discrimination of face-like stimuli by newborn infants. , 1975, Pediatrics.

[21]  C. Gross,et al.  Representations of faces and body parts in macaque temporal cortex: a functional MRI study. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[22]  G. Orban,et al.  Visual Motion Processing Investigated Using Contrast Agent-Enhanced fMRI in Awake Behaving Monkeys , 2001, Neuron.

[23]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Specialization within the ventral stream: the case for the visual word form area , 2004, NeuroImage.

[24]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. , 1983, Child development.

[25]  Irene Leo,et al.  Newborns' face recognition: role of inner and outer facial features. , 2006, Child development.

[26]  Doris Y. Tsao,et al.  Comparing face patch systems in macaques and humans , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[27]  Chen,et al.  A functional magnetic resonance imaging study , 2011 .

[28]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Novel domain formation reveals proto-architecture in inferotemporal cortex , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.

[29]  L. Fogassi,et al.  Neonatal Imitation in Rhesus Macaques , 2006, PLoS biology.

[30]  W. James,et al.  The Principles of Psychology. , 1983 .

[31]  N. Kanwisher Functional specificity in the human brain: A window into the functional architecture of the mind , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[32]  M. Livingstone,et al.  Ocular dominance columns in New World monkeys , 1996, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[33]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge , 1996, Nature.

[34]  Bevil R. Conway,et al.  Parallel, multi-stage processing of colors, faces and shapes in macaque inferior temporal cortex , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.

[35]  M. Livingstone,et al.  A hierarchical, retinotopic proto-organization of the primate visual system at birth , 2017, eLife.

[36]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Development of the macaque face-patch system , 2017, Nature Communications.

[37]  Dwight J. Kravitz,et al.  Cortical representations of bodies and faces are strongest in their commonly experienced configurations , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[38]  D. Hubel,et al.  Comparison of the effects of unilateral and bilateral eye closure on cortical unit responses in kittens. , 1965, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  I. Bushnell,et al.  Mother's face recognition in newborn infants: learning and memory , 2001 .

[40]  R. Turner,et al.  Characterizing Evoked Hemodynamics with fMRI , 1995, NeuroImage.

[41]  M. Pinsk,et al.  Visuotopic Organization of Macaque Posterior Parietal Cortex: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[42]  R. Malach,et al.  The topography of high-order human object areas , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[43]  J. Knott The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory , 1951 .

[44]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Newborns' preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline , 1991, Cognition.

[45]  M. Farah,et al.  Parts and Wholes in Face Recognition , 1993, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[46]  Elias B. Issa,et al.  Precedence of the Eye Region in Neural Processing of Faces , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[47]  D. Heeger,et al.  Spatial attention affects brain activity in human primary visual cortex. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[48]  Wim Vanduffel,et al.  The Retinotopic Organization of Macaque Occipitotemporal Cortex Anterior to V4 and Caudoventral to the Middle Temporal (MT) Cluster , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Involvement of striate and extrastriate visual cortical areas in spatial attention , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[50]  K. Obermayer,et al.  Organization of ocular dominance and orientation columns in the striate cortex of neonatal macaque monkeys , 1995, Visual Neuroscience.

[51]  H. Harlow,et al.  Social recovery by isolation-reared monkeys. , 1971, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[52]  M. Johnson,et al.  Parsing eye-tracking data of variable quality to provide accurate fixation duration estimates in infants and adults , 2012, Behavior research methods.

[53]  Doris Y. Tsao,et al.  Faces and objects in macaque cerebral cortex , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[54]  D. Somers,et al.  Functional MRI reveals spatially specific attentional modulation in human primary visual cortex. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[55]  W. Vanduffel,et al.  Visual Field Map Clusters in Macaque Extrastriate Visual Cortex , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[56]  A. Young,et al.  Configurational Information in Face Perception , 1987, Perception.

[57]  Margaret S Livingstone,et al.  Retinotopic Organization of Scene Areas in Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex , 2017, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[58]  David C. Van Essen,et al.  Application of Information Technology: An Integrated Software Suite for Surface-based Analyses of Cerebral Cortex , 2001, J. Am. Medical Informatics Assoc..

[59]  R W Cox,et al.  AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. , 1996, Computers and biomedical research, an international journal.

[60]  Carlo Umiltà,et al.  Newborns' preference for faces: what is crucial? , 2002, Developmental psychology.

[61]  T. SHALLICE,et al.  Learning and Memory , 1970, Nature.

[62]  Ivo D. Popivanov,et al.  Probabilistic and Single-Subject Retinotopic Maps Reveal the Topographic Organization of Face Patches in the Macaque Cortex , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[63]  Geraint Rees,et al.  Perception and Processing of Faces in the Human Brain Is Tuned to Typical Feature Locations , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[64]  Anders M. Dale,et al.  Repeated fMRI Using Iron Oxide Contrast Agent in Awake, Behaving Macaques at 3 Tesla , 2002, NeuroImage.

[65]  Daniel D. Dilks,et al.  Organization of high-level visual cortex in human infants , 2017, Nature Communications.