Attribute Memory Model and Behavioral Neurophysiology of Memory

Theories have been proposed to account for the multiple memory systems in the brain. Among those, the attribute theory for memory was originally proposed by Kesner. According to the theory, there are six major attributes for defining key aspects of an event. The event, once experienced, leaves its memory in the brain for short term and later becomes consolidated into a long-term memory. The attribute memory model proposes that the former type is processed by an event-based memory system and the latter type is handled by a knowledge-based memory system. The model also proposes a rule-based memory system (which is argued in this chapter as a subsystem of the knowledge-based memory system). The attribute memory model describes how different attributes recruit specific brain regions and how they interact. Computational information processes such as pattern separation and completion have also been proposed by the theory along with their associated subfields in the hippocampus. The model has always been tested in Kesner-style memory tasks, that is, goal-directed memory tasks using positive rewards. Such goal-directed tasks have provided great insights into how different memory systems and associated brain regions work in natural settings. Finding neurophysiological correlates of behavioral measures in these “freely moving” animals, however, poses a great technical challenge when one wants to record single-unit activity at the same time. As a means to continuing the Kesner-style behavioral probing in freely moving animals while analyzing single-unit activity rigorously, one needs some artful modifications in original behavioral paradigms to accommodate both behavioral and electrophysiological components. Once successfully implemented, such modifications would help physiologically test the attribute model of memory.

[1]  R. Kesner Tapestry of memory. , 2009, Behavioral neuroscience.

[2]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Hippocampal conjunctive encoding, storage, and recall: Avoiding a trade‐off , 1994, Hippocampus.

[3]  Raymond P. Kesner,et al.  An analysis of independence and interactions of brain substrates that subserve multiple attributes, memory systems, and underlying processes , 2004, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[4]  E. Tulving,et al.  Episodic and semantic memory , 1972 .

[5]  D Marr,et al.  Simple memory: a theory for archicortex. , 1971, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[6]  R. Kesner,et al.  Long-term memory for contextual attributes: Dissociation of amygdala and hippocampus , 1983, Behavioural Brain Research.

[7]  M. Gluck,et al.  Computational models of the hippocampal region: linking incremental learning and episodic memory , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[8]  R. Kesner,et al.  Memory for spatial locations, motor responses, and objects: triple dissociation among the hippocampus, caudate nucleus, and extrastriate visual cortex , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  A. Treves,et al.  Distinct Ensemble Codes in Hippocampal Areas CA3 and CA1 , 2004, Science.

[10]  Rainer W. Friedrich,et al.  Olfactory pattern classification by discrete neuronal network states , 2010, Nature.

[11]  R. Kesner An attribute analysis of memory: The role of the hippocampus , 1980 .

[12]  Raymond P. Kesner,et al.  The role of the dorsal CA3 hippocampal subregion in spatial working memory and pattern separation , 2006, Behavioural Brain Research.

[13]  E. Tulving How many memory systems are there , 1985 .

[14]  R. Kesner,et al.  Encoding versus retrieval of spatial memory: Double dissociation between the dentate gyrus and the perforant path inputs into CA3 in the dorsal hippocampus , 2004, Hippocampus.

[15]  L. Saksida,et al.  Object memory and perception in the medial temporal lobe: an alternative approach , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[16]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  From Conditioning to Conscious Recollection , 2001 .

[17]  L. Saksida,et al.  Visual perception and memory: a new view of medial temporal lobe function in primates and rodents. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[18]  Dorothy Tse,et al.  References and Notes Supporting Online Material Materials and Methods Figs. S1 to S5 Tables S1 to S3 Electron Impact (ei) Mass Spectra Chemical Ionization (ci) Mass Spectra References Schemas and Memory Consolidation Research Articles Research Articles Research Articles Research Articles , 2022 .

[19]  E. Rolls A theory of hippocampal function in memory , 1996, Hippocampus.

[20]  M. Baxter Involvement of Medial Temporal Lobe Structures in Memory and Perception , 2009, Neuron.

[21]  E. Rolls,et al.  Computational analysis of the role of the hippocampus in memory , 1994, Hippocampus.

[22]  Raymond P. Kesner,et al.  A process analysis of the CA3 subregion of the hippocampus , 2013, Front. Cell. Neurosci..

[23]  M. Packard Glutamate infused posttraining into the hippocampus or caudate-putamen differentially strengthens place and response learning. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[24]  L. Squire Memory systems of the brain: A brief history and current perspective , 2004, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[25]  Inah Lee,et al.  Neural Correlates of Object-in-Place Learning in Hippocampus and Prefrontal Cortex , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  Dorothy Tse,et al.  Schema-Dependent Gene Activation and Memory Encoding in Neocortex , 2011, Science.

[27]  Inah Lee,et al.  Dentate gyrus is necessary for disambiguating similar object-place representations. , 2010, Learning & memory.

[28]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  Free recall and recognition in a network model of the hippocampus: simulating effects of scopolamine on human memory function , 1997, Behavioural Brain Research.

[29]  J. Knierim,et al.  Major Dissociation Between Medial and Lateral Entorhinal Input to Dorsal Hippocampus , 2005, Science.

[30]  C. Stark,et al.  Pattern separation in the hippocampus , 2011, Trends in Neurosciences.

[31]  Donald A Wilson,et al.  Olfactory perceptual stability and discrimination , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  J. Knierim,et al.  Comparison of population coherence of place cells in hippocampal subfields CA1 and CA3 , 2004, Nature.

[33]  Inah Lee,et al.  Differential contribution of NMDA receptors in hippocampal subregions to spatial working memory , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[34]  R. J. McDonald,et al.  A triple dissociation of memory systems: Hippocampus, amygdala, and dorsal striatum. , 1993, Behavioral neuroscience.

[35]  Inah Lee,et al.  Perirhinal cortex is necessary for acquiring, but not for retrieving object-place paired association. , 2010, Learning & memory.

[36]  J. D. McGaugh,et al.  Inactivation of Hippocampus or Caudate Nucleus with Lidocaine Differentially Affects Expression of Place and Response Learning , 1996, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[37]  R. Kesner,et al.  Memory for Magnitude of Reinforcement: Dissociation between the Amygdala and Hippocampus , 1995, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[38]  E. Rolls,et al.  Role of long‐term synaptic modification in short‐term memory , 2001, Hippocampus.

[39]  Wendy A Suzuki,et al.  Perception and the Medial Temporal Lobe: Evaluating the Current Evidence , 2009, Neuron.

[40]  M. Moser,et al.  Pattern Separation in the Dentate Gyrus and CA3 of the Hippocampus , 2007, Science.

[41]  E. Rolls,et al.  A computational theory of hippocampal function, and empirical tests of the theory , 2006, Progress in Neurobiology.

[42]  R P Kesner,et al.  Memory for Spatial Location: Role of the Hippocampus in Mediating Spatial Pattern Separation , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  L. Squire,et al.  The medial temporal lobe memory system , 1991, Science.

[44]  Paul E. Gilbert,et al.  Testing neural network models of memory with behavioral experiments , 2000, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[45]  Lin Tian,et al.  Functional imaging of hippocampal place cells at cellular resolution during virtual navigation , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[46]  Michael R. Hunsaker,et al.  The CA3 subregion of the hippocampus is critical for episodic memory processing by means of relational encoding in rats. , 2008, Behavioral neuroscience.

[47]  Mayank R. Mehta,et al.  Multisensory Control of Hippocampal Spatiotemporal Selectivity , 2013, Science.

[48]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  The hippocampus as an associator of discontiguous events , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.

[49]  L. Saksida,et al.  A computer-automated touchscreen paired-associates learning (PAL) task for mice: impairments following administration of scopolamine or dicyclomine and improvements following donepezil , 2011, Psychopharmacology.

[50]  Inah Lee,et al.  Disconnection of the Hippocampal–Perirhinal Cortical Circuits Severely Disrupts Object–Place Paired Associative Memory , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.