Role of the basal ganglia in category learning: how do patients with Parkinson's disease learn?

The purpose of the present study was to gain a deeper understanding of the role of the basal ganglia in learning and memory by examining learning strategies among patients with basal ganglia dysfunction. Using a probabilistic category learning task (the "weather prediction" task) previously shown to be sensitive to basal ganglia function, the authors examined patterns of performance during learning and used mathematical models to capture different learning strategies. Results showed that patients with Parkinson's disease exhibit different patterns of strategy use. Specifically, most controls initially used a simple, but suboptimal, strategy that focused on single-cue-outcome associations; eventually, however, most controls adopted a more complex, optimal learning strategy, integrating single-cue associations to predict outcomes for multiple-cue stimuli. In contrast, the majority of individuals with Parkinson's disease continued to rely on simple single-cue learning strategies throughout the experiment.

[1]  O. Rehm Beiträge zur Kenntnis des Liqnor cerebrospinalis , 1931, Deutsche Zeitschrift für Nervenheilkunde.

[2]  A. Kleemann Mitteilungen zur Therapie der chronischen Encephalitis , 1929, Deutsche Zeitschrift für Nervenheilkunde.

[3]  M. Gluck,et al.  Impaired probabilistic category learning in hypoxic subjects with hippocampal damage , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  R. Kesner,et al.  Memory for spatial locations, motor responses, and objects: triple dissociation among the hippocampus, caudate nucleus, and extrastriate visual cortex , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[5]  M. Gluck,et al.  Dissociating Hippocampal versus Basal Ganglia Contributions to Learning and Transfer , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[6]  Shawn W. Ell,et al.  Category learning deficits in Parkinson's disease. , 2003, Neuropsychology.

[7]  M. Gluck,et al.  How do people solve the "weather prediction" task?: individual variability in strategies for probabilistic category learning. , 2002, Learning & memory.

[8]  W. Schultz Getting Formal with Dopamine and Reward , 2002, Neuron.

[9]  M. Gluck,et al.  Interactive memory systems in the human brain , 2001, Nature.

[10]  W. T. Maddox,et al.  Striatal contributions to category learning: Quantitative modeling of simple linear and complex nonlinear rule learning in patients with Parkinson's disease , 2001, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[11]  W T Maddox,et al.  A possible role of the striatum in linear and nonlinear category learning: evidence from patients with Huntington's disease. , 2001, Behavioral neuroscience.

[12]  M. Baxter,et al.  Multiple brain-memory systems: the whole does not equal the sum of its parts , 2001, Trends in Neurosciences.

[13]  Richard B. Ivry,et al.  Comparison of the Basal Ganglia and Cerebellum in Shifting Attention. , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  John McDowall,et al.  Preserved Implicit Learning on Both the Serial Reaction Time Task and Artificial Grammar in Patients with Parkinson's Disease , 2001, Brain and Cognition.

[15]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  From Conditioning to Conscious Recollection , 2001 .

[16]  N. Cohen From Conditioning to Conscious Recollection Memory Systems of the Brain. Oxford Psychology Series, Volume 35. , 2001 .

[17]  T. Robbins,et al.  Probabilistic learning and reversal deficits in patients with Parkinson’s disease or frontal or temporal lobe lesions: possible adverse effects of dopaminergic medication , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  J. Horvitz Mesolimbocortical and nigrostriatal dopamine responses to salient non-reward events , 2000, Neuroscience.

[19]  J. Podd,et al.  Recognition memory impairment in Parkinson's disease: power and meta-analyses. , 2000, Neuropsychology.

[20]  C. I. Connolly,et al.  Building neural representations of habits. , 1999, Science.

[21]  M. Packard Glutamate infused posttraining into the hippocampus or caudate-putamen differentially strengthens place and response learning. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[22]  Carol A. Seger,et al.  Striatal activation during acquisition of a cognitive skill. , 1999, Neuropsychology.

[23]  A. Graybiel,et al.  The substantia nigra of the human brain. II. Patterns of loss of dopamine-containing neurons in Parkinson's disease. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[24]  A. Graybiel,et al.  The substantia nigra of the human brain. I. Nigrosomes and the nigral matrix, a compartmental organization based on calbindin D(28K) immunohistochemistry. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  Maddox Wt,et al.  Quantitative modeling of visual attention processes in patients with Parkinson's disease: effects of stimulus integrality on selective attention and dimensional integration. , 1999 .

[26]  L R Squire,et al.  Intact learning of artificial grammars and intact category learning by patients with Parkinson's disease. , 1999, Behavioral neuroscience.

[27]  W T Maddox,et al.  Quantitative modeling of visual attention processes in patients with Parkinson's disease: effects of stimulus integrality on selective attention and dimensional integration. , 1999, Neuropsychology.

[28]  Gregory Ashby,et al.  A neuropsychological theory of multiple systems in category learning. , 1998, Psychological review.

[29]  R. Siegert,et al.  Implicit learning in Parkinson's disease: evidence from a verbal version of the serial reaction time task. , 1998, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[30]  J. Gabrieli Cognitive neuroscience of human memory. , 1998, Annual review of psychology.

[31]  L. Brown,et al.  Sensory and cognitive functions of the basal ganglia , 1997, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[32]  Peter Dayan,et al.  A Neural Substrate of Prediction and Reward , 1997, Science.

[33]  T. Robbins,et al.  Spatial and non-spatial working memory at different stages of Parkinson's disease , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  L. Squire,et al.  Structure and function of declarative and nondeclarative memory systems. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[35]  Jennifer A. Mangels,et al.  A Neostriatal Habit Learning System in Humans , 1996, Science.

[36]  T. Robbins,et al.  Refining the Taxonomy of Memory , 1996, Science.

[37]  M. Koivisto,et al.  Perceptual priming in Alzheimer's and Parkinson's diseases , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[38]  Gurindar S. Sohi,et al.  Memory systems , 1996, CSUR.

[39]  J. D. McGaugh,et al.  Inactivation of Hippocampus or Caudate Nucleus with Lidocaine Differentially Affects Expression of Place and Response Learning , 1996, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[40]  Y. Agid,et al.  Memory for spatial location is affected in Parkinson's disease , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[41]  C. Gerfen,et al.  The frontal cortex-basal ganglia system in primates. , 1996, Critical reviews in neurobiology.

[42]  L. Henderson,et al.  Serial reaction time learning and Parkinson's disease: Evidence for a procedural learning deficit , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  A. Graybiel,et al.  Adaptive neural networks in the basal ganglia. , 1995 .

[44]  Joel L. Davis,et al.  Adaptive Critics and the Basal Ganglia , 1995 .

[45]  P. Strick,et al.  Anatomical evidence for cerebellar and basal ganglia involvement in higher cognitive function. , 1994, Science.

[46]  M. Gluck,et al.  Probabilistic classification learning in amnesia. , 1994, Learning & memory.

[47]  H J Sagar,et al.  Slowed central processing in simple and go/no-go reaction time tasks in Parkinson's disease. , 1994, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[48]  T. Robbins,et al.  Contrasting mechanisms of impaired attentional set-shifting in patients with frontal lobe damage or Parkinson's disease. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[49]  M. Hallett,et al.  Procedural learning in Parkinson's disease and cerebellar degeneration , 1993, Annals of neurology.

[50]  A. M. Owen,et al.  Visuospatial memory deficits at different stages of Parkinson's disease , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[51]  E. Breen Recall and Recognition Memory in Parkinson's Disease , 1993, Cortex.

[52]  Y Agid,et al.  Are dopaminergic neurons selectively vulnerable to Parkinson's disease? , 1993, Advances in neurology.

[53]  R. J. McDonald,et al.  A triple dissociation of memory systems: hippocampus, amygdala, and dorsal striatum. , 1993, Behavioral neuroscience.

[54]  C. Marsden,et al.  Fronto-striatal cognitive deficits at different stages of Parkinson's disease. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[55]  L. Squire Declarative and Nondeclarative Memory: Multiple Brain Systems Supporting Learning and Memory , 1992, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[56]  M. Bondi,et al.  Implicit and explicit memory in Alzheimer's disease and Parkinson's disease. , 1991, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[57]  H J Gundersen,et al.  The absolute number of nerve cells in substantia nigra in normal subjects and in patients with Parkinson's disease estimated with an unbiased stereological method. , 1991, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[58]  M. Moscovitch,et al.  Memory for temporal order and conditional associative-learning in patients with Parkinson's disease , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[59]  E. Marder,et al.  Procedural memory in Parkinson's disease: impaired motor but not visuoperceptual learning. , 1990, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[60]  A. C. Roberts,et al.  Impaired extra-dimensional shift performance in medicated and unmedicated Parkinson's disease: Evidence for a specific attentional dysfunction , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[61]  R. Sutherland,et al.  The hippocampal formation is necessary for rats to learn and remember configural discriminations , 1989, Behavioural Brain Research.

[62]  M. Packard,et al.  Differential effects of fornix and caudate nucleus lesions on two radial maze tasks: evidence for multiple memory systems , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[63]  D. Salmon,et al.  Neuropsychological evidence for multiple implicit memory systems: a comparison of Alzheimer's, Huntington's, and Parkinson's disease patients , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[64]  G. Bower,et al.  From conditioning to category learning: an adaptive network model. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[65]  J. Saint-Cyr,et al.  Procedural learning and neostriatal dysfunction in man. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[66]  G. Robertson,et al.  Evidence that the substantia nigra is a site of action for l-DOPA , 1988, Neuroscience Letters.

[67]  S. Kish,et al.  Uneven pattern of dopamine loss in the striatum of patients with idiopathic Parkinson's disease. Pathophysiologic and clinical implications. , 1988, The New England journal of medicine.

[68]  C. Marsden,et al.  'Frontal' cognitive function in patients with Parkinson's disease 'on' and 'off' levodopa. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[69]  J. Saint-Cyr,et al.  Frontal lobe dysfunction in Parkinson's disease. The cortical focus of neostriatal outflow. , 1986, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[70]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. , 1986, Annual review of neuroscience.

[71]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[72]  B. Djahanguiri,et al.  The prevention of acute gastric ulcer in the rat by alpha-methyldopa. , 1967, Medicina et pharmacologia experimentalis. International journal of experimental medicine.

[73]  A Taxonomy of Memory , .