Brain regions for perceiving and reasoning about other people in school-aged children.
暂无分享,去创建一个
Susan Whitfield-Gabrieli | R. Saxe | K. Pelphrey | S. Whitfield-Gabrieli | Jonathan Scholz | Jonathan Scholz | Rebecca R Saxe | Kevin A Pelphrey
[1] Kevin A. Pelphrey,et al. Grasping the Intentions of Others: The Perceived Intentionality of an Action Influences Activity in the Superior Temporal Sulcus during Social Perception , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[2] Lindsey J. Powell,et al. It's the Thought That Counts , 2006, Psychological science.
[3] N. Kanwisher,et al. The Fusiform Face Area: A Module in Human Extrastriate Cortex Specialized for Face Perception , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[4] M. Kronbichler,et al. Thinking of mental and other representations: The roles of left and right temporo-parietal junction , 2006, Social neuroscience.
[5] C. Frith,et al. Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[6] Laura J. Claxton,et al. Individual differences in executive functioning and theory of mind: An investigation of inhibitory control and planning ability. , 2004, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[7] G. Glover,et al. Cultural and linguistic effects on neural bases of ‘Theory of Mind’ in American and Japanese children , 2007, Brain Research.
[8] A. Leslie. Developmental parallels in understanding minds and bodies , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[9] C. Neil Macrae,et al. The Link between Social Cognition and Self-referential Thought in the Medial Prefrontal Cortex , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[10] J. J. Guajardo,et al. How infants make sense of intentional action , 2001 .
[11] T. Allison,et al. Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[12] Stephanie M. Carlson,et al. The Development of Executive Functioning and Theory of Mind , 2006, Psychological science.
[13] G. McCarthy,et al. When Strangers Pass , 2004, Psychological science.
[14] R. Kohlenberg,et al. FINDING THE SELF: A BEHAVIORAL MEASURE AND ITS CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS , 2001 .
[15] Daniel C. Dennett,et al. Beliefs about beliefs [P&W, SR&B] , 1978, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[16] C. Frith,et al. Interacting minds--a biological basis. , 1999, Science.
[17] H. Wellman,et al. Infant social attention predicts preschool social cognition. , 2004, Developmental science.
[18] Jesper Andersson,et al. Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic , 2005, NeuroImage.
[19] H. Wellman,et al. Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. , 2001, Child development.
[20] T. Allison,et al. Functional anatomy of biological motion perception in posterior temporal cortex: an FMRI study of eye, mouth and hand movements. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.
[21] J. Russell. Justifying all the fuss about false belief , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[22] Alison Gopnik,et al. Children's understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance-reality distinction. , 1988 .
[23] T. Allison,et al. Brain Activity Evoked by the Perception of Human Walking: Controlling for Meaningful Coherent Motion , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[24] Rebecca Saxe,et al. The neural basis of belief encoding and integration in moral judgment , 2008, NeuroImage.
[25] Susan C. Johnson. Detecting agents. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[26] Russell A. Poldrack,et al. In praise of tedious anatomy , 2007, NeuroImage.
[27] M. Tomasello,et al. Chimpanzees understand psychological states – the question is which ones and to what extent , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[28] Aaron C. Koralek,et al. Two Takes on the Social Brain: A Comparison of Theory of Mind Tasks , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[29] K. Pelphrey,et al. School-aged children exhibit domain-specific responses to biological motion , 2006, Social neuroscience.
[30] S. Carey,et al. Understanding other minds: linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging. , 2004, Annual review of psychology.
[31] R. C. Oldfield. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.
[32] R. Saxe,et al. Making sense of another mind: The role of the right temporo-parietal junction , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[33] C. Frith,et al. Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[34] D. Sperber,et al. PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Attribution of Beliefs by 13-Month-Old Infants , 2022 .
[35] G. Csibra,et al. Action Anticipation Through Attribution of False Belief by 2-Year-Olds , 2007, Psychological science.
[36] K. Grill-Spector,et al. Differential development of high-level visual cortex correlates with category-specific recognition memory , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.
[37] Ori Friedman,et al. Core mechanisms in ‘theory of mind’ , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[38] G. Rizzolatti,et al. The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.
[39] T. Ruffman,et al. Does eye gaze indicate implicit knowledge of false belief? Charting transitions in knowledge. , 2001, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[40] Rebecca Saxe,et al. Reading minds versus following rules: Dissociating theory of mind and executive control in the brain , 2006, Social neuroscience.
[41] V. Gallese. The manifold nature of interpersonal relations: the quest for a common mechanism. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[42] Z. Nadasdy,et al. Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age , 1995, Cognition.
[43] A. Woodward. Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach , 1998, Cognition.
[44] N. Kanwisher,et al. Can generic expertise explain special processing for faces? , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[45] Sunny Shin,et al. Do 15-Month-Old Infants Understand False Beliefs ? , 2005 .
[46] Nancy Kanwisher,et al. Divide and conquer: A defense of functional localizers , 2006, NeuroImage.
[47] P. Bloom,et al. Two reasons to abandon the false belief task as a test of theory of mind , 2000, Cognition.
[48] J. Perner,et al. The Curious Incident of the Photo that was Accused of Being False: Issues of Domain Specificity in Development, Autism, and Brain Imaging , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[49] G. Glover,et al. Cultural and linguistic influence on neural bases of ‘Theory of Mind’: An fMRI study with Japanese bilinguals , 2006, Brain and Language.
[50] A. Meltzoff,et al. Like me” as a building block for understanding other minds: Bodily acts, attention, and intention. Ed. Malle, BF, L. J. Moses, and DA Baldwin , 2001 .
[51] C. Frith,et al. Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.
[52] R Saxe,et al. People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.
[53] G. Glover,et al. Children's and adults’ neural bases of verbal and nonverbal ‘theory of mind’ , 2007, Neuropsychologia.
[54] G. Shulman,et al. Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[55] A. Leslie,et al. Solving belief problems: toward a task analysis , 1998, Cognition.
[56] A. Gopnik,et al. Children's understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance-reality distinction. , 1988, Child development.
[57] J. Perner,et al. Development of theory of mind and executive control , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[58] T. Allison,et al. Temporal Cortex Activation in Humans Viewing Eye and Mouth Movements , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[59] C. Neil Macrae,et al. General and specific contributions of the medial prefrontal cortex to knowledge about mental states , 2005, NeuroImage.
[60] Kevin N. Ochsner,et al. The neural correlates of direct and reflected self-knowledge , 2005, NeuroImage.
[61] Dare A. Baldwin,et al. Intentions and Intentionality: Foundations of Social Cognition , 2001 .
[62] Thomas F. Nugent,et al. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[63] M. Brass,et al. Investigating Action Understanding: Inferential Processes versus Action Simulation , 2007, Current Biology.
[64] Aina Puce,et al. Electrophysiology and brain imaging of biological motion. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[65] N. Kanwisher,et al. Common Neural Substrates for Response Selection across Modalities and Mapping Paradigms , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[66] D. Perrett,et al. A region of right posterior superior temporal sulcus responds to observed intentional actions , 2004, Neuropsychologia.
[67] E. Darcy Burgund,et al. Comparison of functional activation foci in children and adults using a common stereotactic space , 2003, NeuroImage.
[68] G. Morgan,et al. Nicaraguan Sign Language and Theory of Mind: the issue of critical periods and abilities. , 2006, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.
[69] R. Saxe. Uniquely human social cognition , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[70] C. N. Macrae,et al. Finding the Self? An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[71] Abraham Z. Snyder,et al. The Feasibility of a Common Stereotactic Space for Children and Adults in fMRI Studies of Development , 2002, NeuroImage.
[72] Michael S. Gazzaniga,et al. “ Theory of Mind ” as a Mechanism of Selective Attention , 2001 .