An Event-related fMRI Study of Artificial Grammar Learning in a Balanced Chunk Strength Design

Artificial grammar learning (Reber, 1967) is a form of implicit learning in which cognitive, rather than motor, implicit learning has been found. After viewing a series of letter strings formed according to a finite state rule system, people are able to classify new letter strings as to whether or not they are formed according to these grammatical rules despite little conscious insight into the rule structure. Previous research has shown that these classification judgments are based on knowledge of abstract rules as well as superficial similarity (chunk strength) to training strings. Here we used event-related fMRI to identify neural regions involved in using both sources of information as test stimuli were designed to unconfound chunk strength from rule use. Using functional connectivity analyses, the extent to which the sources of information are complementary or competitive was also assessed. Activation in the right caudate was associated with rule adherence, whereas medial temporal lobe activations were associated with chunk strength. Additionally, functional connectivity analyses revealed caudate and medial temporal lobe activations to be strongly negatively correlated (r = 88) with one another during the performance of this task.

[1]  A. Reber Implicit learning of artificial grammars , 1967 .

[2]  A. Reber Implicit learning of synthetic languages: The role of instructional set. , 1976 .

[3]  L. Squire,et al.  Preserved learning and retention of pattern-analyzing skill in amnesia: dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. , 1980, Science.

[4]  R. Rosenthal Meta-analytic procedures for social research , 1984 .

[5]  J. Becker,et al.  Dissociations between skill learning and verbal recognition in amnesia and dementia. , 1984, Archives of neurology.

[6]  A. Lang,et al.  Striatal dopamine distribution in Parkinsonian patients during life , 1985, Journal of the Neurological Sciences.

[7]  T. McMillan,et al.  Neuropsychological assessment of neuropsychiatric disorders , 1987 .

[8]  M. Nissen,et al.  Implicit learning in patients with probable Alzheimer's disease , 1987, Neurology.

[9]  Roger C. Schank,et al.  SCRIPTS, PLANS, GOALS, AND UNDERSTANDING , 1988 .

[10]  Andrew J. Saykin,et al.  Neuropsychological Assessment of Neuropsychiatric Disorders , 1988 .

[11]  M. Packard,et al.  Differential effects of fornix and caudate nucleus lesions on two radial maze tasks: evidence for multiple memory systems , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[12]  D. Salmon,et al.  Neuropsychological evidence for multiple implicit memory systems: a comparison of Alzheimer's, Huntington's, and Parkinson's disease patients , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[13]  C. Marsden,et al.  The performance on learning tasks of patients in the early stages of Parkinson's disease , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  Pierre Perruchet,et al.  Synthetic grammar learning: Implicit rule abstraction or explicit fragmentary knowledge? Journal of , 1990 .

[15]  E. Marder,et al.  Procedural memory in Parkinson's disease: impaired motor but not visuoperceptual learning. , 1990, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[16]  John R. Vokey,et al.  Abstract analogies and abstracted grammars: Comments on Reber (1989) and Mathews et al. (1989). , 1991 .

[17]  D Broadbent,et al.  Implicit and explicit knowledge bases in artificial grammar learning. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[18]  M. Nissen,et al.  Procedural learning is impaired in Huntington's disease: Evidence from the serial reaction time task , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[19]  W. Swann,et al.  Outcasts in a white-lie society: the enigmatic worlds of people with negative self-conceptions. , 1992, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[20]  Stephen M. Kosslyn,et al.  Is Cognitive Neuropsychology Plausible? The Perils of Sitting on a One-Legged Stool , 1992, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[21]  Daniel B. Willingham,et al.  Evidence for dissociable motor skills in Huntington’s disease patients , 1993, Psychobiology.

[22]  D. Balota,et al.  Implicit Memory and the Formation of New Associations in Nondemented Parkinson′s Disease Individuals and Individuals with Senile Dementia of the Alzheimer Type: A Serial Reaction Time (SRT) Investigation , 1993, Brain and Cognition.

[23]  N. Ambady,et al.  Half a minute: Predicting teacher evaluations from thin slices of nonverbal behavior and physical attractiveness. , 1993 .

[24]  Carol A. Seger,et al.  Implicit learning. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.

[25]  L R Squire,et al.  The information acquired during artificial grammar learning. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[26]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  A PET investigation of implicit and explicit sequence learning , 1995 .

[27]  Niels Birbaumer,et al.  Memory and Skill Acquisition in Parkinson's Disease and Frontal Lobe Dysfunction , 1995, Cortex.

[28]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Functional Mapping of Sequence Learning in Normal Humans , 1995, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[29]  D. Shanks IMPLICIT LEARNING AND TACIT KNOWLEDGE - AN ESSAY ON THE COGNITIVE UNCONSCIOUS - REBER,A , 1995 .

[30]  L. Henderson,et al.  Serial reaction time learning and Parkinson's disease: Evidence for a procedural learning deficit , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[31]  Joel L. Davis,et al.  Adaptive Critics and the Basal Ganglia , 1995 .

[32]  J. Gabrieli Contribution of the basal ganglia to skill learning and working memory in humans , 1995 .

[33]  L. Squire,et al.  Artificial grammar learning depends on implicit acquisition of both abstract and exemplar-specific information. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[34]  Jane S. Paulsen,et al.  Dissociations within nondeclarative memory in Huntington's disease. , 1996 .

[35]  Jennifer A. Mangels,et al.  A Neostriatal Habit Learning System in Humans , 1996, Science.

[36]  Thierry Meulemans,et al.  Associative chunk strength in artificial grammar learning , 1997 .

[37]  S. Rauch,et al.  Striatal recruitment during an implicit sequence learning task as measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging , 1997, Human brain mapping.

[38]  Carol A. Seger,et al.  Two Forms of Sequential Implicit Learning , 1997, Consciousness and Cognition.

[39]  G. Berns,et al.  Brain regions responsive to novelty in the absence of awareness. , 1997, Science.

[40]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Attention and stimulus characteristics determine the locus of motor-sequence encoding. A PET study. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[41]  P. Reber,et al.  Contrasting cortical activity associated with category memory and recognition memory. , 1998, Learning & memory.

[42]  K. J. Plasterk Louis Costa and The Journal of Clinical (and Experimental) Neuropsychology , 1998 .

[43]  R L Buckner,et al.  Functional neuroimaging studies of encoding, priming, and explicit memory retrieval. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[44]  P. Ashworth,et al.  Handbook of Implicit Learning , 1998 .

[45]  F. Ashby,et al.  On the nature of implicit categorization , 1999, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[46]  D. Rubin,et al.  Contrasts and Effect Sizes in Behavioral Research , 1999 .

[47]  Carol A. Seger,et al.  Striatal activation during acquisition of a cognitive skill. , 1999, Neuropsychology.

[48]  R. J. McDonald,et al.  Effects of medial and lateral caudate-putamen lesions on place- and cue-guided behaviors in the water maze: relation to thigmotaxis , 1999, Behavioural Brain Research.

[49]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Stochastic Designs in Event-Related fMRI , 1999, NeuroImage.

[50]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Learning-related neuronal responses in prefrontal cortex studied with functional neuroimaging. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[51]  P. Peigneux,et al.  Exploration of implicit artificial grammar learning in Parkinson's disease. , 1999, Acta neurologica Belgica.

[52]  L R Squire,et al.  Intact learning of artificial grammars and intact category learning by patients with Parkinson's disease. , 1999, Behavioral neuroscience.

[53]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  Intuition: a social cognitive neuroscience approach. , 2000, Psychological bulletin.

[54]  Carol A. Seger,et al.  Neural activity differs between explicit and implicit learning of artificial grammar strings: An fMRI study , 2000, Psychobiology.

[55]  John McDowall,et al.  Preserved Implicit Learning on Both the Serial Reaction Time Task and Artificial Grammar in Patients with Parkinson's Disease , 2001, Brain and Cognition.

[56]  D. Schacter,et al.  Perceptual specificity in visual object priming: functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for a laterality difference in fusiform cortex , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[57]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  Why introverts can't always tell who likes them: multitasking and nonverbal decoding. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[58]  M. Gluck,et al.  Interactive memory systems in the human brain , 2001, Nature.

[59]  R. J. Boik Contrasts and Effect Sizes in Behavioral Research: A Correlational Approach , 2001 .

[60]  G Deuschl,et al.  Intact artificial grammar learning in patients with cerebellar degeneration and advanced Parkinson’s disease , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[61]  B. Knowlton,et al.  Learning and memory functions of the Basal Ganglia. , 2002, Annual review of neuroscience.

[62]  Carol A. Seger,et al.  Striatal activity in concept learning , 2002, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[63]  Paul J. Reber,et al.  Neural Correlates of Artificial Grammar Learning , 2002, NeuroImage.

[64]  J. Saint-Cyr Frontal-striatal circuit functions: Context, sequence, and consequence , 2003, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[65]  Shawn W. Ell,et al.  Category learning deficits in Parkinson's disease. , 2003, Neuropsychology.

[66]  Tanya L. Chartrand,et al.  Consequences of automatic goal pursuit and the case of nonconscious mimicry. , 2003 .

[67]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  Evidence-based and intuition-based self-knowledge: an FMRI study. , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[68]  Barbara J Knowlton,et al.  Visual feature learning in artificial grammar classification. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[69]  B. Knowlton,et al.  An implicit learning task activates medial temporal lobe in patients with Parkinson's disease. , 2004, Behavioral neuroscience.

[70]  Wayne W Manges,et al.  Some radical implications of Bach-y-Rita's discoveries. , 2005, Journal of integrative neuroscience.

[71]  Jutta L. Mueller,et al.  Native and Nonnative Speakers' Processing of a Miniature Version of Japanese as Revealed by ERPs , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[72]  K. Petersson,et al.  Towards an explicit account of implicit learning , 2005, Current opinion in neurology.

[73]  John R. Vokey,et al.  Salience of Item Knowledge in Learning Artificial Grammars , 1992 .