The Neural Basis of Event Simulation: An fMRI Study

Event simulation (ES) is the situational inference process in which perceived event features such as objects, agents, and actions are associated in the brain to represent the whole situation. ES provides a common basis for various cognitive processes, such as perceptual prediction, situational understanding/prediction, and social cognition (such as mentalizing/trait inference). Here, functional magnetic resonance imaging was used to elucidate the neural substrates underlying important subdivisions within ES. First, the study investigated whether ES depends on different neural substrates when it is conducted explicitly and implicitly. Second, the existence of neural substrates specific to the future-prediction component of ES was assessed. Subjects were shown contextually related object pictures implying a situation and performed several picture–word-matching tasks. By varying task goals, subjects were made to infer the implied situation implicitly/explicitly or predict the future consequence of that situation. The results indicate that, whereas implicit ES activated the lateral prefrontal cortex and medial/lateral parietal cortex, explicit ES activated the medial prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, and medial/lateral temporal cortex. Additionally, the left temporoparietal junction plays an important role in the future-prediction component of ES. These findings enrich our understanding of the neural substrates of the implicit/explicit/predictive aspects of ES-related cognitive processes.

[1]  E. Denkova,et al.  Experiencing past and future personal events: Functional neuroimaging evidence on the neural bases of mental time travel , 2008, Brain and Cognition.

[2]  Kevin N. Ochsner,et al.  The neural correlates of direct and reflected self-knowledge , 2005, NeuroImage.

[3]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Grasping the Intentions of Others with One's Own Mirror Neuron System , 2005, PLoS biology.

[4]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  J. Decety,et al.  Effect of subjective perspective taking during simulation of action: a PET investigation of agency , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[6]  K. Szpunar,et al.  Neural substrates of envisioning the future , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[7]  Jonathan Evans Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[8]  D. Schacter,et al.  On the nature of medial temporal lobe contributions to the constructive simulation of future events , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[9]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  Integrating automatic and controlled processes into neurocognitive models of social cognition , 2006, Brain Research.

[10]  Melina R. Uncapher,et al.  Posterior parietal cortex and episodic retrieval: convergent and divergent effects of attention and memory. , 2009, Learning & memory.

[11]  M. Ferguson,et al.  Beyond behaviorism: on the automaticity of higher mental processes. , 2000, Psychological bulletin.

[12]  D. Schacter,et al.  Constructive episodic simulation of the future and the past: Distinct subsystems of a core brain network mediate imagining and remembering , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  Mirella Dapretto,et al.  Developmental changes in the neural basis of interpreting communicative intent. , 2006, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[14]  M. Hallett,et al.  Modeling other minds , 1995, Neuroreport.

[15]  Elizabeth Jefferies,et al.  Elucidating the Nature of Deregulated Semantic Cognition in Semantic Aphasia: Evidence for the Roles of Prefrontal and Temporo-parietal Cortices , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[16]  R Saxe,et al.  People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.

[17]  Lawrence W Barsalou,et al.  Simulation, situated conceptualization, and prediction , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[18]  J. Decety,et al.  What you believe versus what you think they believe: a neuroimaging study of conceptual perspective‐taking , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[19]  K. Zilles,et al.  Mind Reading: Neural Mechanisms of Theory of Mind and Self-Perspective , 2001, NeuroImage.

[20]  C. Neil Macrae,et al.  The Link between Social Cognition and Self-referential Thought in the Medial Prefrontal Cortex , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[21]  O. Blanke,et al.  Multisensory Mechanisms in Temporo-Parietal Cortex Support Self-Location and First-Person Perspective , 2011, Neuron.

[22]  M. Corbetta,et al.  The Reorienting System of the Human Brain: From Environment to Theory of Mind , 2008, Neuron.

[23]  J. Grafman,et al.  Structured event complexes in the medial prefrontal cortex support counterfactual representations for future planning , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[24]  S. Fiske,et al.  The Handbook of Social Psychology , 1935 .

[25]  Angela R. Laird,et al.  Across-study and within-subject functional connectivity of a right temporo-parietal junction subregion involved in stimulus–context integration , 2012, NeuroImage.

[26]  C. Frith,et al.  Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’ , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[27]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Neural substrates of narrative comprehension and memory , 2008, NeuroImage.

[28]  C. Neil Macrae,et al.  General and specific contributions of the medial prefrontal cortex to knowledge about mental states , 2005, NeuroImage.

[29]  D. Hassabis,et al.  Deconstructing episodic memory with construction , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[30]  K. Szpunar,et al.  Contextual processing in episodic future thought. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[31]  F. Overwalle Social cognition and the brain: a meta-analysis. , 2009 .

[32]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Frontal and Temporo-Parietal Lobe Contributions to Theory of Mind: Neuropsychological Evidence from a False-Belief Task with Reduced Language and Executive Demands , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[33]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Distinct neural systems subserve person and object knowledge , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[34]  Uta Frith,et al.  Implicit and Explicit Processes in Social Cognition , 2008, Neuron.

[35]  Uri Hasson,et al.  Brain networks subserving the extraction of sentence information and its encoding to memory. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[36]  Daniel Houser,et al.  A functional imaging study of cooperation in two-person reciprocal exchange , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[37]  Demis Hassabis,et al.  Differential engagement of brain regions within a ‘core’ network during scene construction , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[38]  Demis Hassabis,et al.  The construction system of the brain , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[39]  R. Nathan Spreng,et al.  The Common Neural Basis of Autobiographical Memory, Prospection, Navigation, Theory of Mind, and the Default Mode: A Quantitative Meta-analysis , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[40]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Detecting Activations in PET and fMRI: Levels of Inference and Power , 1996, NeuroImage.

[41]  D. Schacter,et al.  Constructive episodic simulation: Temporal distance and detail of past and future events modulate hippocampal engagement , 2008, Hippocampus.

[42]  J. Decety,et al.  A PET Investigation of the Attribution of Intentions with a Nonverbal Task , 2000, NeuroImage.

[43]  R. Dolan,et al.  How the Brain Translates Money into Force: A Neuroimaging Study of Subliminal Motivation , 2007, Science.

[44]  G. Gallup,et al.  Where am I? The neurological correlates of self and other. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[45]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  Social cognitive neuroscience: a review of core processes. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.

[46]  Colin Humphries,et al.  Syntactic and Semantic Modulation of Neural Activity during Auditory Sentence Comprehension , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[47]  C. Urgesi,et al.  The Spiritual Brain: Selective Cortical Lesions Modulate Human Self-Transcendence , 2010, Neuron.

[48]  Tilo Kircher,et al.  Neural correlates of narrative shifts during auditory story comprehension , 2009, NeuroImage.

[49]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  Dissociable effects of prefrontal and anterior temporal cortical lesions on stereotypical gender attitudes , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[50]  M. Bar,et al.  Cortical Analysis of Visual Context , 2003, Neuron.

[51]  D. Wegner,et al.  Control and automaticity in social life. , 1998 .

[52]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: an fMRI study , 1999, The European journal of neuroscience.

[53]  Elizabeth Jefferies,et al.  Both the Middle Temporal Gyrus and the Ventral Anterior Temporal Area Are Crucial for Multimodal Semantic Processing: Distortion-corrected fMRI Evidence for a Double Gradient of Information Convergence in the Temporal Lobes , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[54]  Dylan D Wagner,et al.  Individual Differences in the Spontaneous Recruitment of Brain Regions Supporting Mental State Understanding When Viewing Natural Social Scenes , 2011 .

[55]  Alana T. Wong,et al.  Remembering the past and imagining the future: Common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[56]  J. Bargh The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. , 1994 .

[57]  Evelyn C. Ferstl,et al.  The role of coherence and cohesion in text comprehension: an event-related fMRI study. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[58]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  The medial prefrontal cortex mediates social event knowledge , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[59]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Medial prefrontal dissociations during processing of trait diagnostic and nondiagnostic person information. , 2006, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[60]  Christopher D. Frith,et al.  Imaging the Intentional Stance in a Competitive Game , 2002, NeuroImage.

[61]  R. Saxe,et al.  Making sense of another mind: The role of the right temporo-parietal junction , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[62]  Evelyn C. Ferstl,et al.  What Does the Frontomedian Cortex Contribute to Language Processing: Coherence or Theory of Mind? , 2002, NeuroImage.

[63]  R. Buckner,et al.  Self-projection and the brain , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[64]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Dual-process theories in social psychology , 1999 .

[65]  Moshe Bar,et al.  The proactive brain: memory for predictions , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[66]  P. Devine Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. , 1989 .

[67]  Colin Humphries,et al.  Time course of semantic processes during sentence comprehension: An fMRI study , 2007, NeuroImage.

[68]  D. Perrett,et al.  Being the target of another’s emotion: a PET study , 2003, Neuropsychologia.