Ventromedial prefrontal volume predicts understanding of others and social network size

Cognitive abilities such as Theory of Mind (ToM), and more generally mentalizing competences, are central to human sociality. Neuroimaging has associated these abilities with specific brain regions including temporo-parietal junction, superior temporal sulcus, frontal pole, and ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Previous studies have shown both that mentalizing competence, indexed as the ability to correctly understand others' belief states, is associated with social network size and that social group size is correlated with frontal lobe volume across primate species (the social brain hypothesis). Given this, we predicted that both mentalizing competences and the number of social relationships a person can maintain simultaneously will be a function of gray matter volume in these regions associated with conventional Theory of Mind. We used voxel-based morphometry of Magnetic Resonance Images (MRIs) to test this hypothesis in humans. Specifically, we regressed individuals' mentalizing competences and social network sizes against gray matter volume. This revealed that gray matter volume in bilateral posterior frontal pole and left temporoparietal junction and superior temporal sucus varies parametrically with mentalizing competence. Furthermore, gray matter volume in the medial orbitofrontal cortex and the ventral portion of medial frontal gyrus, varied parametrically with both mentalizing competence and social network size, demonstrating a shared neural basis for these very different facets of sociality. These findings provide the first fine-grained anatomical support for the social brain hypothesis. As such, they have important implications for our understanding of the constraints limiting social cognition and social network size in humans, as well as for our understanding of how such abilities evolved across primates.

[1]  T. Shallice From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure: Converging Operations: Specific Syndromes and Evidence from Normal Subjects , 1988 .

[2]  Robin I. M. Dunbar Evolutionary Basis of the Social Brain , 2011 .

[3]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  EVIDENCE FOR COEVOLUTION OF SOCIALITY AND RELATIVE BRAIN SIZE IN THREE ORDERS OF MAMMALS , 2007, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.

[4]  Michael B. Miller,et al.  Neural Correlates of Detecting Pretense: Automatic Engagement of the Intentional Stance under Covert Conditions , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[5]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  Social networks, support cliques, and kinship , 1995, Human nature.

[6]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  Understanding primate brain evolution , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[7]  A. Leslie Pretending and believing: issues in the theory of ToMM , 1994, Cognition.

[8]  James Stiller,et al.  Perspective-taking and memory capacity predict social network size , 2007, Soc. Networks.

[9]  Elliot Saltzman,et al.  Skilled actions: a task-dynamic approach. , 1987, Psychological review.

[10]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  Species differences in executive function correlate with hippocampus volume and neocortex ratio across nonhuman primates. , 2010, Journal of comparative psychology.

[11]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  Social network size in humans , 2003, Human nature.

[12]  R. Nathan Spreng,et al.  Patterns of Brain Activity Supporting Autobiographical Memory, Prospection, and Theory of Mind, and Their Relationship to the Default Mode Network , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  Matcheri S. Keshavan,et al.  Volumetric alterations of the orbitofrontal cortex in autism , 2007, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry.

[14]  Candy Rowe,et al.  A critique of comparative studies of brain size , 2007, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[15]  R. Buckner,et al.  Self-projection and the brain , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[16]  C. Buys,et al.  Human Sympathy Groups , 1979 .

[17]  C. Frith,et al.  Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[18]  Francesca Baglio,et al.  Effects of aging on mindreading ability through the eyes: An fMRI study , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[19]  M. Rushworth,et al.  The contribution of distinct subregions of the ventromedial frontal cortex to emotion, social behavior, and decision making , 2008, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[20]  C. Frith,et al.  The Neural Basis of Mentalizing , 2006, Neuron.

[21]  M. Kendall Statistical Methods for Research Workers , 1937, Nature.

[22]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  Darwin and the ghost of Phineas Gage: Neuro-evolution and the social brain , 2009, Cortex.

[23]  D. Stuss,et al.  The frontal lobes are necessary for 'theory of mind'. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[24]  Lindsey J. Powell,et al.  It's the Thought That Counts , 2006, Psychological science.

[25]  A. Leslie Pretense and representation: The origins of "theory of mind." , 1987 .

[26]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  Theory-of-mind deficits and causal attributions , 1998 .

[27]  Szabolcs Kéri,et al.  Emotion-related brain regions. , 2008, Ideggyogyaszati szemle.

[28]  George A. Miller From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure.Tim Shallice , 1990 .

[29]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  The evolution of the social brain: anthropoid primates contrast with other vertebrates , 2007, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[30]  M Coltheart From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure. Tim Shallice. Cambridge University Press, New York, 1988. xvi, 462 pp., illus. $59.50; paper, $24.95. , 1989, Science.

[31]  Uta Frith,et al.  Theory of mind , 2001, Current Biology.

[32]  S. Carey,et al.  Understanding other minds: linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging. , 2004, Annual review of psychology.

[33]  N. Rempel-Clower,et al.  Role of Orbitofrontal Cortex Connections in Emotion , 2007, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[34]  Beatriz Luna,et al.  Combining Brains: A Survey of Methods for Statistical Pooling of Information , 2002, NeuroImage.

[35]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A Voxel-Based Morphometric Study of Ageing in 465 Normal Adult Human Brains , 2001, NeuroImage.

[36]  Beate Sodian,et al.  Neural correlates of true and false belief reasoning , 2007, NeuroImage.

[37]  Didier Sornette,et al.  Discrete hierarchical organization of social group sizes , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[38]  Hanneke E. M. den Ouden,et al.  Thinking about intentions , 2005, NeuroImage.

[39]  F. Overwalle Social cognition and the brain: a meta-analysis. , 2009 .

[40]  Henrik Walter,et al.  Understanding Intentions in Social Interaction: The Role of the Anterior Paracingulate Cortex , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  Thomas V. Pollet,et al.  Exploring variation in active network size: Constraints and ego characteristics , 2009, Soc. Networks.

[42]  William A. Cunningham,et al.  Tools of the Trade Type I and Type II error concerns in fMRI research : rebalancing the scale , 2009 .

[43]  J. Decety,et al.  The Role of the Right Temporoparietal Junction in Social Interaction: How Low-Level Computational Processes Contribute to Meta-Cognition , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[44]  C. Frith,et al.  Brain regions involved in prospective memory as determined by positron emission tomography , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[45]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Automatic Differentiation of Anatomical Patterns in the Human Brain: Validation with Studies of Degenerative Dementias , 2002, NeuroImage.

[46]  M. Kronbichler,et al.  Thinking of mental and other representations: The roles of left and right temporo-parietal junction , 2006, Social neuroscience.

[47]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception , 1983, Cognition.

[48]  J. Astington,et al.  The child''s discovery of mind , 1994 .

[49]  William A. Cunningham,et al.  Type I and Type II error concerns in fMRI research: re-balancing the scale. , 2009, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[50]  Michael E. Pereira,et al.  Primate life histories and socioecology , 2003 .

[51]  John W. Tukey,et al.  Statistical Methods for Research Workers , 1930, Nature.

[52]  R. Nathan Spreng,et al.  The Common Neural Basis of Autobiographical Memory, Prospection, Navigation, Theory of Mind, and the Default Mode: A Quantitative Meta-analysis , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[53]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  Orbital prefrontal cortex volume correlates with social cognitive competence , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[54]  Gereon R Fink,et al.  Structural brain abnormalities in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder and patients with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. , 2007, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[55]  C. Frith,et al.  Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[56]  Shanshan Hong,et al.  Voxel-based morphometry study on brain structure in children with high-functioning autism , 2008, Neuroreport.

[57]  T. Rogers,et al.  Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[58]  I. Wilkinson,et al.  Investigating the functional anatomy of empathy and forgiveness , 2001, Neuroreport.

[59]  S. Fekete,et al.  Regional gray matter reduction and theory of mind deficit in the early phase of schizophrenia: a voxel‐based morphometric study , 2009, Acta psychiatrica Scandinavica.

[60]  Uta Frith,et al.  The Biological Basis of Social Interaction , 2001 .

[61]  R Saxe,et al.  People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.

[62]  E. Rolls Functions of the orbitofrontal and pregenual cingulate cortex in taste, olfaction, appetite and emotion. , 2008, Acta physiologica Hungarica.

[63]  Tetsuya Matsuda,et al.  Brain activation associated with evaluative processes of guilt and embarrassment: an fMRI study , 2004, NeuroImage.

[64]  Dunbar Rim.,et al.  Mind the gap; or why humans are not just great apes , 2008 .

[65]  Bruce T. Milne,et al.  The complex structure of hunter–gatherer social networks , 2007, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[66]  Antonio Y Hardan,et al.  An MRI study of increased cortical thickness in autism. , 2006, The American journal of psychiatry.

[67]  Rebecca Elliott,et al.  Neuronal correlates of theory of mind and empathy: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study in a nonverbal task , 2006, NeuroImage.

[68]  J. Aharon-Peretz,et al.  Two systems for empathy: a double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions. , 2009, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[69]  T. Shallice,et al.  Confidence in Recognition Memory for Words: Dissociating Right Prefrontal Roles in Episodic Retrieval , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[70]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  Encephalization is not a universal macroevolutionary phenomenon in mammals but is associated with sociality , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[71]  Robin I. M. Dunbar Social Brain Hypothesis , 1998, Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science.

[72]  C. Frith,et al.  Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’ , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[73]  Rebecca Saxe,et al.  Why and how to study Theory of Mind with fMRI , 2006, Brain Research.

[74]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  Functional Networks in Emotional Moral and Nonmoral Social Judgments , 2002, NeuroImage.