Paying attention to social meaning: an FMRI study.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Rebecca Saxe,et al. Dissociation between emotion and personality judgments: Convergent evidence from functional neuroimaging , 2005, NeuroImage.
[2] David Premack,et al. Origins of human social competence. , 1995 .
[3] Geoffrey Bird,et al. Attention does not modulate neural responses to social stimuli in autism spectrum disorders , 2006, NeuroImage.
[4] Patrice D. Tremoulet,et al. Perceptual causality and animacy , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[5] J. Cacioppo,et al. On seeing human: a three-factor theory of anthropomorphism. , 2007, Psychological review.
[6] Jason P. Mitchell. Activity in right temporo-parietal junction is not selective for theory-of-mind. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.
[7] R. Cabeza,et al. Imaging Cognition II: An Empirical Review of 275 PET and fMRI Studies , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[8] T. Heatherton,et al. Detecting agency from the biological motion of veridical vs animated agents. , 2007, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.
[9] Daniel Tranel,et al. Amygdala damage impairs emotional memory for gist but not details of complex stimuli , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[10] Kenji Kawano,et al. Global and fine information coded by single neurons in the temporal visual cortex , 1999, Nature.
[11] P. Barnard,et al. Autobiographical memory and daily schemas at work. , 1994, Memory.
[12] Kiyotaka Nemoto,et al. The neural network for the mirror system and mentalizing in normally developed children: an fMRI study , 2004, Neuroreport.
[13] Alan Kingstone,et al. The eyes have it!: An fMRI investigation , 2004, Brain and Cognition.
[14] Juha Silvanto,et al. Stimulation of the human frontal eye fields modulates sensitivity of extrastriate visual cortex. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.
[15] A P Leff,et al. Impaired reading in patients with right hemianopia , 2000, Annals of neurology.
[16] E. Maguire,et al. The functional neuroanatomy of comprehension and memory: the importance of prior knowledge. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[17] Karl J. Friston,et al. A unified statistical approach for determining significant signals in images of cerebral activation , 1996, Human brain mapping.
[18] Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al. Human Brain Activity Time-Locked to Narrative Event Boundaries , 2007, Psychological science.
[19] J. Duncan,et al. Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.
[20] James L. McClelland,et al. The parallel distributed processing approach to semantic cognition , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[21] P. Todd,et al. Accurate judgments of intention from motion cues alone: A cross-cultural study , 2005 .
[22] Marcus E Raichle,et al. Intrinsic brain activity sets the stage for expression of motivated behavior , 2005, The Journal of comparative neurology.
[23] F. Heider,et al. An experimental study of apparent behavior , 1944 .
[24] R. Dolan,et al. The Kuleshov Effect: the influence of contextual framing on emotional attributions. , 2006, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.
[25] Jordan Grafman,et al. Social concepts are represented in the superior anterior temporal cortex , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[26] Thomas M. Moerland,et al. Fate of unattended fearful faces in the amygdala is determined by both attentional resources and cognitive modulation , 2005, NeuroImage.
[27] Alison J. Wiggett,et al. Behavioral / Systems / Cognitive Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Investigation of Overlapping Lateral Occipitotemporal Activations Using Multi-Voxel Pattern Analysis , 2006 .
[28] Ralph Adolphs,et al. Impaired spontaneous anthropomorphizing despite intact perception and social knowledge. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[29] N. Tzourio-Mazoyer,et al. Automated Anatomical Labeling of Activations in SPM Using a Macroscopic Anatomical Parcellation of the MNI MRI Single-Subject Brain , 2002, NeuroImage.
[30] Christopher D. Frith,et al. Imaging the Intentional Stance in a Competitive Game , 2002, NeuroImage.
[31] Jordan Grafman,et al. Handbook of Neuropsychology , 1991 .
[32] C. Frith,et al. Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[33] Bridgette A. Martin,et al. Segmenting Ambiguous Events , 2003 .
[34] James K Rilling,et al. The neural correlates of theory of mind within interpersonal interactions , 2004, NeuroImage.
[35] Chris Rorden,et al. Spatial Normalization of Brain Images with Focal Lesions Using Cost Function Masking , 2001, NeuroImage.
[36] B. Levine,et al. The functional neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: A meta-analysis , 2006, Neuropsychologia.
[37] Matthew Brett,et al. An Evaluation of the Use of Magnetic Field Maps to Undistort Echo-Planar Images , 2003, NeuroImage.
[38] Jodie A. Baird,et al. Discerning intentions in dynamic human action , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[39] P. Skudlarski,et al. The role of the fusiform face area in social cognition: implications for the pathobiology of autism. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[40] D. Amaral,et al. The Amygdala, Social Behavior, and Danger Detection , 2003, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[41] Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al. Human brain activity time-locked to perceptual event boundaries , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.
[42] G. V. Simpson,et al. Preparatory deployment of attention to motion activates higher-order motion-processing brain regions , 2004, NeuroImage.
[43] Alex Martin,et al. NEURAL FOUNDATIONS FOR UNDERSTANDING SOCIAL AND MECHANICAL CONCEPTS , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[44] T. Allison,et al. Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[45] Mitsuo Kawato,et al. Activation of the Human Superior Temporal Gyrus during Observation of Goal Attribution by Intentional Objects , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[46] Istvan Molnar-Szakacs,et al. Watching social interactions produces dorsomedial prefrontal and medial parietal BOLD fMRI signal increases compared to a resting baseline , 2004, NeuroImage.
[47] A. Meltzoff,et al. The detection of contingency and animacy from simple animations in the human brain. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.
[48] R. Cabeza,et al. Imaging Cognition: An Empirical Review of PET Studies with Normal Subjects , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[49] Philip J. Barnard,et al. Affect, Cognition and Change: Re-Modelling Depressive Thought , 1993 .
[50] P. Cavanagh,et al. Cortical fMRI activation produced by attentive tracking of moving targets. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.
[51] E. Bizzi,et al. The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1996 .
[52] R. Saxe. Uniquely human social cognition , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[53] Daniel Houser,et al. A functional imaging study of cooperation in two-person reciprocal exchange , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[54] Darren Newtson. An Interactionist Perspective on Social Knowing , 1980 .
[55] Mahzarin R. Banaji,et al. Encoding-Specific Effects of Social Cognition on the Neural Correlates of Subsequent Memory , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[56] Anthony R. McIntosh,et al. Parallel networks operating across attentional deployment and motion processing: A multi-seed partial least squares fMRI study , 2006, NeuroImage.
[57] S. G. Cox,et al. Functional MRI study of the cognitive generation of affect. , 1999, The American journal of psychiatry.
[58] S. Fiske,et al. Comprar Social Beings: A Core Motives Approach to Social Psychology | Benjamin L. Miller | 9780470129111 | Wiley , 2009 .
[59] C. Frith,et al. Movement and Mind: A Functional Imaging Study of Perception and Interpretation of Complex Intentional Movement Patterns , 2000, NeuroImage.
[60] Shawn C. Milleville,et al. Understanding Animate Agents , 2007, Psychological science.
[61] M. Corbetta,et al. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[62] Pascal Boyer,et al. How the brain perceives causality: an event-related fMRI study , 2001, Neuroreport.
[63] B. Parkinson. Ideas and realities of emotion , 1995 .
[64] C. Frith,et al. Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[65] J. Bargh. What have we been priming all these years? On the development, mechanisms, and ecology of nonconscious social behavior. , 2006, European journal of social psychology.
[66] G. Glover,et al. Reflecting upon Feelings: An fMRI Study of Neural Systems Supporting the Attribution of Emotion to Self and Other , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[67] Joseph T. Devlin,et al. Orienting attention to semantic categories , 2006, NeuroImage.
[68] C. Frith,et al. How we predict what other people are going to do , 2006, Brain Research.
[69] M. Behrmann,et al. Seeing it differently: visual processing in autism , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[70] Dharshan Kumaran,et al. The Human Hippocampus: Cognitive Maps or Relational Memory? , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[71] R. Buckner,et al. Self-projection and the brain , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[72] U. Frith,et al. Vagaries of Visual Perception in Autism , 2005, Neuron.
[73] J. Lancaster,et al. Using the talairach atlas with the MNI template , 2001, NeuroImage.