Distinct neural patterns of social cognition for cooperation versus competition

How do people consider other minds during cooperation versus competition? Some accounts predict that theory of mind (ToM) is recruited more for cooperation versus competition or competition versus cooperation, whereas other accounts predict similar recruitment across these two contexts. The present fMRI study examined activity in brain regions for ToM (bilateral temporoparietal junction, precuneus, dorsomedial prefrontal cortex) across cooperative and competitive interactions with the same individual within the same paradigm. Although univariate analyses revealed that ToM regions overall were recruited similarly across interaction contexts, multivariate pattern analyses revealed that these regions nevertheless encoded information separating cooperation from competition. Specifically, ToM regions encoded differences between cooperation and competition when people believed the outcome was determined by their and their partner's choices but not when the computer determined the outcome. We propose that, when people are motivated to consider others' mental states, ToM regions encode different aspects of mental states during cooperation versus competition. Given the role of these regions for ToM, these findings reveal distinct patterns of social cognition for distinct motivational contexts.

[1]  N. Kriegeskorte,et al.  Neural correlates of trust , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[2]  Carey K. Morewedge,et al.  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Making Sense by Making Sentient : Effectance Motivation Increases , 2010 .

[3]  D. Wegner,et al.  Dimensions of Mind Perception , 2007, Science.

[4]  F. Cushman Crime and punishment: Distinguishing the roles of causal and intentional analyses in moral judgment , 2008, Cognition.

[5]  R. Saxe,et al.  Decoding moral judgments from neural representations of intentions , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[6]  J. V. Bavel,et al.  Group membership alters the threshold for mind perception: The role of social identity, collective identification, and intergroup threat , 2014 .

[7]  Marcel Brass,et al.  How social is error observation? The neural mechanisms underlying the observation of human and machine errors. , 2014, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[8]  C. Frith,et al.  Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  L. Young,et al.  Two motivations for two dimensions of mind , 2014 .

[10]  Tom M. Mitchell,et al.  Machine learning classifiers and fMRI: A tutorial overview , 2009, NeuroImage.

[11]  James K Rilling,et al.  The neural correlates of theory of mind within interpersonal interactions , 2004, NeuroImage.

[12]  D. Hassabis,et al.  Foraging under Competition: The Neural Basis of Input-Matching in Humans , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  R. Saxe,et al.  The neural basis of the interaction between theory of mind and moral judgment , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[14]  Herbert G. Kelman Violence without Moral Restraint: Reflections on the Dehumanization of Victims and Victimizers , 1973 .

[15]  N. Haslam,et al.  Dehumanization and infrahumanization. , 2014, Annual review of psychology.

[16]  D. Wegner,et al.  Causes and consequences of mind perception , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[17]  Andrew N. Meltzoff,et al.  The neural bases of cooperation and competition: an fMRI investigation , 2004, NeuroImage.

[18]  A. Galinsky,et al.  Why It Pays to Get Inside the Head of Your Opponent The Differential Effects of Perspective Taking and Empathy in Strategic Interactions , 2022 .

[19]  J. Hart,et al.  Distinct prefrontal cortex activity associated with item memory and source memory for visual shapes. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[20]  Michael V. Lombardo,et al.  Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from developmental social neuroscience , 2013 .

[21]  D. Wegner,et al.  What do I think you're doing? Action identification and mind attribution. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[22]  Emile G. Bruneau,et al.  Us and Them: Intergroup Failures of Empathy , 2011 .

[23]  M. Kronbichler,et al.  Thinking of mental and other representations: The roles of left and right temporo-parietal junction , 2006, Social neuroscience.

[24]  Penelope L. Mavros,et al.  Impaired theory of mind for moral judgment in high-functioning autism , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[25]  Rajeev D. S. Raizada,et al.  Smoothness without Smoothing: Why Gaussian Naive Bayes Is Not Naive for Multi-Subject Searchlight Studies , 2013, PloS one.

[26]  L. Young,et al.  Motive attribution asymmetry for love vs. hate drives intractable conflict , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[27]  A. Ishai,et al.  The Gender of Face Stimuli is Represented in Multiple Regions in the Human Brain , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[28]  Rebecca Elliott,et al.  Co-operation with another player in a financially rewarded guessing game activates regions implicated in theory of mind , 2006, Social neuroscience.

[29]  Simon B. Eickhoff,et al.  A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data , 2005, NeuroImage.

[30]  Susan T. Fiske,et al.  Attention and weight in person perception: The impact of negative and extreme behavior. , 1980 .

[31]  David G. Rand,et al.  Humans Display a 'Cooperative Phenotype' that is Domain General and Temporally Stable , 2014 .

[32]  R Saxe,et al.  People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.

[33]  F. Cushman Deconstructing intent to reconstruct morality , 2015 .

[34]  G. Peeters,et al.  Positive-Negative Asymmetry in Evaluations: The Distinction Between Affective and Informational Negativity Effects , 1990 .

[35]  Anthony D. Wagner,et al.  Detecting individual memories through the neural decoding of memory states and past experience , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[36]  Carey K. Morewedge Negativity bias in attribution of external agency. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[37]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Origins of human cooperation and morality. , 2013, Annual review of psychology.

[38]  Jeroen Vaes,et al.  The Emotional Side of Prejudice: The Attribution of Secondary Emotions to Ingroups and Outgroups , 2000 .

[39]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  A Three-Dimensional Statistical Analysis for CBF Activation Studies in Human Brain , 1992, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[40]  S. Fiske,et al.  Social groups that elicit disgust are differentially processed in mPFC. , 2007, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[41]  Laurie R Santos,et al.  Ecology, Domain Specificity, and the Origins of Theory of Mind: Is Competition the Catalyst? , 2006 .

[42]  S. Huettel,et al.  A Distinct Role of the Temporal-Parietal Junction in Predicting Socially Guided Decisions , 2012, Science.

[43]  R. W. White Motivation reconsidered: the concept of competence. , 1959, Psychological review.

[44]  Yasuhito Sawahata,et al.  Spatial smoothing hurts localization but not information: Pitfalls for brain mappers , 2010, NeuroImage.

[45]  L. Young,et al.  Theory of mind for processing unexpected events across contexts. , 2016, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[46]  M. Nowak,et al.  A strategy of win-stay, lose-shift that outperforms tit-for-tat in the Prisoner's Dilemma game , 1993, Nature.

[47]  Jay W. Jackson,et al.  Realistic group conflict theory: A review and evaluation of the theoretical and empirical literature. , 1993 .

[48]  Xu Cui,et al.  NIRS-based hyperscanning reveals increased interpersonal coherence in superior frontal cortex during cooperation , 2012, NeuroImage.

[49]  A. Woodward,et al.  Not all emotions are created equal: the negativity bias in social-emotional development. , 2008, Psychological bulletin.

[50]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  On seeing human: a three-factor theory of anthropomorphism. , 2007, Psychological review.

[51]  Anthony D Wagner,et al.  Goal-Directed Modulation of Neural Memory Patterns: Implications for fMRI-Based Memory Detection , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[52]  Christopher R. Chartier,et al.  Use of social knowledge in tacit coordination: Social focal points , 2014 .

[53]  Kurt Gray,et al.  Moral typecasting: divergent perceptions of moral agents and moral patients. , 2009, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[54]  N. Kriegeskorte,et al.  Revealing representational content with pattern-information fMRI--an introductory guide. , 2009, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[55]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Chimpanzees are more skilful in competitive than in cooperative cognitive tasks , 2004, Animal Behaviour.

[56]  Christopher D. Frith,et al.  Imaging the Intentional Stance in a Competitive Game , 2002, NeuroImage.

[57]  J. Call,et al.  The psychology of primate cooperation and competition: a call for realigning research agendas , 2016, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[58]  Peter Bossaerts,et al.  Neural correlates of mentalizing-related computations during strategic interactions in humans , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[59]  Brian Hare,et al.  Can competitive paradigms increase the validity of experiments on primate social cognition? , 2001, Animal Cognition.

[60]  R. Saxe,et al.  Disruption of the right temporoparietal junction with transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces the role of beliefs in moral judgments , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[61]  Rebecca Saxe,et al.  The neural basis of belief encoding and integration in moral judgment , 2008, NeuroImage.

[62]  Jean Decety,et al.  The speed of morality: a high-density electrical neuroimaging study. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[63]  Dimitri Van De Ville,et al.  Decoding of Emotional Information in Voice-Sensitive Cortices , 2009, Current Biology.

[64]  M. Taborsky Cooperation built the Tower of Babel , 2007, Behavioural Processes.

[65]  G. Juckel,et al.  Cooperation and Deception Recruit Different Subsets of the Theory-of-Mind Network , 2008, PloS one.

[66]  S. Fiske,et al.  Intentional Harms Are Worse, Even When They’re Not , 2013, Psychological science.

[67]  Stephen M. Garcia,et al.  Being Unpredictable , 2010 .

[68]  Susan V. Opotow Moral Exclusion and Injustice: An Introduction , 1990 .

[69]  Daniel Houser,et al.  A functional imaging study of cooperation in two-person reciprocal exchange , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[70]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Neural signatures of strategic types in a two-person bargaining game , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[71]  Rebecca Saxe,et al.  An fMRI Investigation of Spontaneous Mental State Inference for Moral Judgment , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[72]  Rebecca Saxe,et al.  fMRI item analysis in a theory of mind task , 2011, NeuroImage.

[73]  R. Adolphs Conceptual Challenges and Directions for Social Neuroscience , 2010, Neuron.

[74]  S. Schwartz,et al.  Intergroup aggression: its predictors and distinctness from in-group bias. , 1989, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[75]  Adam Waytz,et al.  Mind Attribution is for Morality , 2013 .

[76]  T. Ethofer,et al.  Decoding of emotional information in voice-sensitive cortices , 2009, NeuroImage.

[77]  W. Hamilton,et al.  The Evolution of Cooperation , 1984 .

[78]  Jay J Van Bavel,et al.  Is race erased? Decoding race from patterns of neural activity when skin color is not diagnostic of group boundaries. , 2013, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[79]  Aaron C. Koralek,et al.  Two Takes on the Social Brain: A Comparison of Theory of Mind Tasks , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[80]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension , 1995, Cognition.

[81]  Ronald Noë Cooperation experiments: coordination through communication versus acting apart together , 2006, Animal Behaviour.

[82]  Yarrow Dunham,et al.  Group bias in cooperative norm enforcement , 2016, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[83]  N. Haslam Dehumanization: An Integrative Review , 2006, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[84]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Mentalizing under Uncertainty: Dissociated Neural Responses to Ambiguous and Unambiguous Mental State Inferences , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[85]  Rebecca Saxe,et al.  When minds matter for moral judgment: intent information is neurally encoded for harmful but not impure acts. , 2016, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[86]  Michael Esterman,et al.  Decoding Task-based Attentional Modulation during Face Categorization , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[87]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Assessing the significance of focal activations using their spatial extent , 1994, Human brain mapping.

[88]  D. Fudenberg,et al.  Tit-for-tat or win-stay, lose-shift? , 2007, Journal of theoretical biology.

[89]  A. Griffin,et al.  Social semantics : altruism , cooperation , mutualism , strong reciprocity and group selection , 2007 .

[90]  G. Stasser,et al.  Coordination success and interpersonal perceptions: matching versus mismatching. , 2008, Journal of personality and social psychology.