Roles of attention and form in visual motion processing : Psychophysical and brain imaging studies

INTRODUCTION It goes without saying that the human visual information processing system is very complex. Marr (1982) proposed that several principles should be taken into consideration to improve understanding of this complex system. These include the modularity principle and the feedforward principle. The modularity principle assumes that the visual system consists of several processes that are relatively independent of each other in early stages of visual processing (frontend processing). Therefore, each of these processes can be largely examined independently without considering the others. The feedforward principle assumes that the majority of visual functions can be understood without taking feedback effects into consideration. While research that follows these principles has greatly contributed to understanding some important aspects of visual information processing, other important aspects of information processing have been relatively neglected, especially, interactions between different processes and feedback from higher to lower levels (however, see Grossberg & Mingolla, 1985; Koch & Davis, 1994). Research on motion perception is not exceptional in this sense. Over the last decade, as in other aspects of visual processing, the majority of research on the processing of velocity of a moving object has mainly explored feedforward processing within the motion module (for a review, Hildreth & Koch, 1987; Nakayama, 1985; Snowden, 1992).

[1]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Surface decomposition accompanying the perception of transparency. , 1993, Spatial vision.

[2]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Attentional modulation of adaptation to two-component transparent motion , 1995, Vision Research.

[3]  Jacob Beck,et al.  Perception of transparency in man and machine , 1985, Comput. Vis. Graph. Image Process..

[4]  George Sperling,et al.  Attention-generated apparent motion , 1995, Nature.

[5]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Directional Motion Sensitivity under Transparent Motion Conditions , 1996, Vision Research.

[6]  K. Tanaka,et al.  Underlying mechanisms of the response specificity of expansion/contraction and rotation cells in the dorsal part of the medial superior temporal area of the macaque monkey. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Effect of surface medium on visual search for orientation and size features. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  Thomas D. Albright,et al.  Neural correlates of perceptual motion coherence , 1992, Nature.

[9]  R A Andersen,et al.  The response of area MT and V1 neurons to transparent motion , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[10]  J. Movshon,et al.  Selectivity for orientation and direction of motion of single neurons in cat striate and extrastriate visual cortex. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  A. Yuille,et al.  A Theoretical Framework for Visual Motion , 1996 .

[12]  J. Wolfe,et al.  A purely binocular mechanism in human vision , 1981, Vision Research.

[13]  I. THE ATTENTION SYSTEM OF THE HUMAN BRAIN , 2002 .

[14]  Hugh R. Wilson,et al.  Perceived direction of moving two-dimensional patterns depends on duration, contrast and eccentricity , 1992, Vision Research.

[15]  Norberto M. Grzywacz,et al.  A computational theory for the perception of coherent visual motion , 1988, Nature.

[16]  T. Poggio,et al.  A parallel algorithm for real-time computation of optical flow , 1989, Nature.

[17]  S. Grossberg,et al.  Neural dynamics of form perception: boundary completion, illusory figures, and neon color spreading. , 1985, Psychological review.

[18]  Darren Burke,et al.  The contribution of one-dimensional motion mechanisms to the perceived direction of drifting plaids and their aftereffects , 1994, Vision Research.

[19]  R. Desimone,et al.  Selective attention gates visual processing in the extrastriate cortex. , 1985, Science.

[20]  J. Beck Perception of transparency in man and machine , 1985, Computer Vision Graphics and Image Processing.

[21]  M. Bravo,et al.  Preattentive Vision and Perceptual Groups , 1990, Perception.

[22]  Stephen Grossberg,et al.  How is a moving target continuously tracked behind occluding cover , 1997 .

[23]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Selective and divided attention during visual discriminations of shape, color, and speed: functional anatomy by positron emission tomography , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[24]  T. D. Albright,et al.  Transparency and coherence in human motion perception , 1990, Nature.

[25]  D J Heeger,et al.  Model for the extraction of image flow. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[26]  P Cavanagh,et al.  Attention-based motion perception. , 1992, Science.

[27]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Attentional modulation of visual motion processing in cortical areas MT and MST , 1996, Nature.

[28]  B. C. Motter Focal attention produces spatially selective processing in visual cortical areas V1, V2, and V4 in the presence of competing stimuli. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  Michael S. Landy,et al.  Theories for the Visual Perception of Local Velocity and Coherent Motion , 1991 .

[30]  D. C. Van Essen,et al.  Concurrent processing streams in monkey visual cortex , 1988, Trends in Neurosciences.

[31]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields of single neurones in the cat's striate cortex , 1959, The Journal of physiology.

[32]  S. Grossberg,et al.  Cortical dynamics of visual motion perception: short-range and long-range apparent motion. , 1992, Psychological review.

[33]  Leslie Welch,et al.  The perception of moving plaids reveals two motion-processing stages , 1989, Nature.

[34]  R. Snowden The perception of visual motion , 1992, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[35]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Assimilation and contrast in motion perception: Explorations in cooperativity , 1990, Vision Research.

[36]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex , 1962, The Journal of physiology.

[37]  E. Adelson,et al.  The analysis of moving visual patterns , 1985 .

[38]  James T. Todd,et al.  The perception of globally coherent motion , 1992, Vision Research.

[39]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Movement aftereffect of bi-vectorial transparent motion , 1994, Vision Research.

[40]  R A Andersen,et al.  Hierarchical processing of motion in the visual cortex of monkey. , 1990, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.

[41]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Motion capture of luminance stimuli by equiluminous color gratings and by attentive tracking , 1994, Vision Research.

[42]  A Gorea,et al.  Directional performances with moving plaids: component-related and plaid-related processing modes coexist. , 1991, Spatial vision.

[43]  Richard I. Ivry,et al.  The perception of transparency with achromatic colors , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.

[44]  Ellen C. Hildreth,et al.  Measurement of Visual Motion , 1984 .

[45]  Joel L. Davis,et al.  Large-Scale Neuronal Theories of the Brain , 1994 .

[46]  E. Adelson,et al.  Phenomenal coherence of moving visual patterns , 1982, Nature.

[47]  A. Chaudhuri Modulation of the motion aftereffect by selective attention , 1990, Nature.

[48]  A. Treisman,et al.  A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[49]  Takeo Watanabe,et al.  The role of parsing in high level motion processing , 1998 .

[50]  E H Adelson,et al.  Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.