Considerations arising from a complementary learning systems perspective on hippocampus and neocortex

We discuss a framework for the organization of learning systems in the mammalian brain, in which the hippocampus and related areas form a memory system complementary to learning mechanisms in neocortex and other areas. The hippocampal system stores new episodes and “replays” them to the neocortical system, interleaved with ongoing experience, allowing generalization as cortical memories form. The data to account for include: 1) neurophysiological findings concerning representations in hippocampal areas, 2) behavioral evidence demonstrating a spatial role for hippocampus, 3) and effects of surgical and pharmacological manipulations on neuronal firing in hippocampal regions in behaving animals. We hypothesize that the hippocampal memory system consists of three major modules: 1) an invertible encoder subsystem supported by the pathways between neocortex and entorhinal cortex, which provides a stable, compressed, invertible encoding in entorhinal cortex (EC) of cortical activity patterns, 2) a memory separation, storage, and retrieval subsystem, supported by pathways between EC, dentate gyrus and area CA3, including the CA3 recurrent collaterals, which facilitates encoding and storage in CA3 of individual EC patterns, and retrieval of those CA3 encodings, in a manner that minimizes interference, and 3) a memory decoding subsystem, supported by the Shaffer collaterals from area CA1 to area CA3 and the bi‐directional pathways between EC and CA3, which provides the means by which a retrieved CA3 coding of an EC pattern can reinstate that pattern on EC. This model has shown that 1) there is a trade‐off between the need for information‐preserving, structure‐extracting encoding of cortical traces and the need for effective storage and recall of arbitrary traces, 2) long‐term depression of synaptic strength in the pathways subject to long‐term potentiation is crucial in preserving information, 3) area CA1 must be able to exploit correlations in EC patterns in the direct perforant path synapses. © 1997 Wiley‐Liss, Inc.

[1]  D. Marr A theory of cerebellar cortex , 1969, The Journal of physiology.

[2]  D Marr,et al.  Simple memory: a theory for archicortex. , 1971, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[3]  J. O'Keefe,et al.  The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. , 1971, Brain research.

[4]  David Willshaw,et al.  Models of distributed associative memory , 1971 .

[5]  T. Bliss,et al.  Long‐lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the unanaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path , 1973, The Journal of physiology.

[6]  T. Bliss,et al.  Long‐lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path , 1973, The Journal of physiology.

[7]  L. Nadel,et al.  The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map , 1978 .

[8]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Synaptic enhancement in fascia dentata: Cooperativity among coactive afferents , 1978, Brain Research.

[9]  W. Levy,et al.  Synapses as associative memory elements in the hippocampal formation , 1979, Brain Research.

[10]  R. Passingham The hippocampus as a cognitive map J. O'Keefe & L. Nadel, Oxford University Press, Oxford (1978). 570 pp., £25.00 , 1979, Neuroscience.

[11]  T. H. Brown,et al.  Associative long-term potentiation in hippocampal slices. , 1983, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[12]  T. H. Brown,et al.  Voltage-clamp analysis of mossy fiber synaptic input to hippocampal neurons. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  A Learning Algorithm for Boltzmann Machines , 1985, Cogn. Sci..

[14]  T. Teyler,et al.  The hippocampal memory indexing theory. , 1986, Behavioral neuroscience.

[15]  T. Teyler,et al.  The hippocampal memory indexing theory. , 1986, Behavioral neuroscience.

[16]  James L. McClelland Resource requirements of standard and programmable nets , 1986 .

[17]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Hippocampal synaptic enhancement and information storage within a distributed memory system , 1987, Trends in Neurosciences.

[18]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Preserved spatial coding in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells during reversible suppression of CA3c output: evidence for pattern completion in hippocampus , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[19]  Gordon H. Bower,et al.  Computational models of learning in simple neural systems , 1989 .

[20]  L. Squire,et al.  Autobiographical memory and amnesia , 1989 .

[21]  G. Buzsáki Two-stage model of memory trace formation: A role for “noisy” brain states , 1989, Neuroscience.

[22]  W. Levy A computational approach to hippocampal function , 1989 .

[23]  Antonio R. Damasio,et al.  The Brain Binds Entities and Events by Multiregional Activation from Convergence Zones , 1989, Neural Computation.

[24]  D. Amaral,et al.  Neurons, numbers and the hippocampal network. , 1990, Progress in brain research.

[25]  L. Squire,et al.  The primate hippocampal formation: evidence for a time-limited role in memory storage. , 1990, Science.

[26]  G. Winocur Anterograde and retrograde amnesia in rats with dorsal hippocampal or dorsomedial thalamic lesions , 1990, Behavioural Brain Research.

[27]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Comparison of spatial and temporal characteristics of neuronal activity in sequential stages of hippocampal processing. , 1990, Progress in brain research.

[28]  Edmund T. Rolls,et al.  What determines the capacity of autoassociative memories in the brain? Network , 1991 .

[29]  Max R. Bennett,et al.  Probabilistic secretion of quanta in the central nervous system: granule cell synaptic control of pattern separation and activity regulation. , 1991, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[30]  O. Brousse Generativity and systematicity in neural network combinatorial learning , 1992 .

[31]  M. Fanselow,et al.  Modality-specific retrograde amnesia of fear. , 1992, Science.

[32]  N. Schmajuk,et al.  Stimulus configuration, classical conditioning, and hippocampal function. , 1992, Psychological review.

[33]  L. Squire "Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans": Correction. , 1992 .

[34]  R. Muller,et al.  The positional firing properties of medial entorhinal neurons: description and comparison with hippocampal place cells , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[35]  E T Rolls,et al.  Computational constraints suggest the need for two distinct input systems to the hippocampal CA3 network , 1992, Hippocampus.

[36]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Spatial selectivity of unit activity in the hippocampal granular layer , 1993, Hippocampus.

[37]  L. Jarrard On the role of the hippocampus in learning and memory in the rat. , 1993, Behavioral and neural biology.

[38]  J. O’Keefe,et al.  Phase relationship between hippocampal place units and the EEG theta rhythm , 1993, Hippocampus.

[39]  M. Gluck,et al.  Hippocampal mediation of stimulus representation: A computational theory , 1993, Hippocampus.

[40]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  Laminar selectivity of the cholinergic suppression of synaptic transmission in rat hippocampal region CA1: computational modeling and brain slice physiology , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[41]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Hippocampal conjunctive encoding, storage, and recall: Avoiding a trade‐off , 1994, Hippocampus.

[42]  P Alvarez,et al.  Memory consolidation and the medial temporal lobe: a simple network model. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[43]  E. Rolls,et al.  Computational analysis of the role of the hippocampus in memory , 1994, Hippocampus.

[44]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep. , 1994, Science.

[45]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  Two functional components of the hippocampal memory system , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[46]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. , 1995, Psychological review.

[47]  R. Sutherland,et al.  Configural association theory and the hippocampal formation: An appraisal and reconfiguration , 1995, Hippocampus.

[48]  W E Skaggs,et al.  Deciphering the hippocampal polyglot: the hippocampus as a path integration system. , 1996, The Journal of experimental biology.

[49]  M. Gluck,et al.  Integrating behavioral and physiological models of hippocampal function , 1996, Hippocampus.

[50]  K M Gothard,et al.  Binding of hippocampal CA1 neural activity to multiple reference frames in a landmark-based navigation task , 1996, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[51]  C. Buhusi,et al.  Attention, configuration, and hippocampal function , 1996, Hippocampus.

[52]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Theta phase precession in hippocampal neuronal populations and the compression of temporal sequences , 1996, Hippocampus.

[53]  J. Murre TraceLink: A model of amnesia and consolidation of memory , 1996, Hippocampus.

[54]  W B Levy,et al.  A sequence predicting CA3 is a flexible associator that learns and uses context to solve hippocampal‐like tasks , 1996, Hippocampus.

[55]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  Encoding and retrieval of episodic memories: Role of cholinergic and GABAergic modulation in the hippocampus , 1998, Hippocampus.