Chapter 11 – Psychobiological Models of Hippocampal Function in Learning and Memory1

Publisher Summary Many models and theories have been proposed over the past few decades that attempt to characterize the role of the hippocampal region in learning and memory. The anterograde amnesia that follows human hippocampal region damage is most saliently characterized by an inability to acquire new episodic information, the kind of information about individual events and experiences that is accessible to conscious control. The aspect of autoassociative memory systems seems ideal for implementing a spatial processor, in which the broad memory of a place should be evoked by any of several views of the area, even if some of the usual cues are missing. Perhaps most pervasive is the idea that the fast, temporary storage in an autoassociator is an important component of an episodic or declarative memory system, in which arbitrary patterns are stored. This chapter emphasizes that in model simulations, self-regulated suppression of recurrent collaterals does suffice to allow switching between storage and recall states in an autoassociative network. It reviews several computational models of hippocampal function in learning and memory.

[1]  J Ambros-Ingerson,et al.  Simulation of paleocortex performs hierarchical clustering. , 1990, Science.

[2]  M. Gluck,et al.  Integrating behavioral and physiological models of hippocampal function , 1996, Hippocampus.

[3]  C. Buhusi,et al.  Attention, configuration, and hippocampal function , 1996, Hippocampus.

[4]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep. , 1994, Science.

[5]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Hippocampal conjunctive encoding, storage, and recall: Avoiding a trade‐off , 1994, Hippocampus.

[6]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  A Computational Model of Cholinergic Disruption of Septohippocampal Activity in Classical Eyeblink Conditioning , 1996, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[7]  H. T. Blair,et al.  Stimulus configuration, spatial learning, and hippocampal function , 1993, Behavioural Brain Research.

[8]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  On the Binding of Associations in Memory: Clues From Studies on the Role of the Hippocampal Region in Paired-Associate Learning , 1995 .

[9]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Considerations arising from a complementary learning systems perspective on hippocampus and neocortex , 1996, Hippocampus.

[10]  M. Gluck,et al.  Hippocampal mediation of stimulus representation: A computational theory , 1993, Hippocampus.

[11]  J S Liaw,et al.  Dynamic synapse: A new concept of neural representation and computation , 1996, Hippocampus.

[12]  Wayne A. Wickelgren,et al.  Chunking and consolidation: A theoretical synthesis of semantic networks configuring in conditioning , 1979 .

[13]  H. Eichenbaum The Hippocampal System and Declarative Memory in Animals , 1992, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  L. Squire,et al.  Retrograde amnesia and memory consolidation: a neurobiological perspective , 1995, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[15]  J. Gray Memory buffer and comparator can share the same circuitry , 1985, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[16]  G. Buzsáki Two-stage model of memory trace formation: A role for “noisy” brain states , 1989, Neuroscience.

[17]  M. Gluck,et al.  Cortico-hippocampal representations in simultaneous odor discrimination: a computational interpretation of Eichenbaum, Mathews, and Cohen (1989). , 1996, Behavioral neuroscience.

[18]  T. Bliss,et al.  Long‐lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path , 1973, The Journal of physiology.

[19]  W. Levy A computational approach to hippocampal function , 1989 .

[20]  R U Muller,et al.  Hippocampal place cells connected by Hebbian synapses can solve spatial problems , 1996, Hippocampus.

[21]  R. Hirsh The hippocampus and contextual retrieval of information from memory: a theory. , 1974, Behavioral biology.

[22]  W. Levy,et al.  Partial quantification of the associative synaptic learning rule of the dentate gyrus , 1983, Neuroscience.

[23]  D J Willshaw,et al.  An assessment of Marr's theory of the hippocampus as a temporary memory store. , 1990, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[24]  I. Daum,et al.  Classical conditioning in patients with severe memory problems. , 1989, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[25]  J. Theios,et al.  Acquisition and extinction of a classically conditioned response in hippocampectomized rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus). , 1972, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[26]  B. Volpe,et al.  Amnesia following the rupture and repair of an anterior communicating artery aneurysm. , 1983, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[27]  Francis Crick,et al.  The function of dream sleep , 1983, Nature.

[28]  M. Fanselow,et al.  Modality-specific retrograde amnesia of fear. , 1992, Science.

[29]  M A Gluck,et al.  Computational models of the neural bases of learning and memory. , 1993, Annual review of neuroscience.

[30]  M. Recce,et al.  Memory for places: A navigational model in support of Marr's theory of hippocampal function , 1996, Hippocampus.

[31]  Geoffrey E. Hinton Connectionist Learning Procedures , 1989, Artif. Intell..

[32]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  Hippocampal system dysfunction and odor discrimination learning in rats: impairment or facilitation depending on representational demands. , 1988, Behavioral neuroscience.

[33]  R. Llinás,et al.  Entorhinal cortex long-term potentiation evoked by theta-patterned stimulation of associative fibers in the isolated in vitro guinea pig brain , 1993, Brain Research.

[34]  C. Köhler Intrinsic connections of the retrohippocampal region in the rat brain. II. The medial entorhinal area , 1986, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[35]  H. T. Blair,et al.  Neural network modeling of the hippocampal formation spatial signals and their possible role in navigation: A modular approach , 1996, Hippocampus.

[36]  L. Squire,et al.  Neuroanatomy of memory. , 1993, Annual review of neuroscience.

[37]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  Encoding and retrieval of episodic memories: Role of cholinergic and GABAergic modulation in the hippocampus , 1998, Hippocampus.

[38]  E. Warrington,et al.  Conditioning in amnesic patients , 1979, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  Michael Recce,et al.  A model of hippocampal function , 1994, Neural Networks.

[40]  M. Hasselmo Neuromodulation and cortical function: modeling the physiological basis of behavior , 1995, Behavioural Brain Research.

[41]  L. Squire,et al.  Memory and amnesia: resistance to disruption develops for years after learning. , 1979, Behavioral and neural biology.

[42]  J. Price An autoradiographic study of complementary laminar patterns of termination of afferent fibers to the olfactory cortex , 1973, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[43]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Hippocampal synaptic enhancement and information storage within a distributed memory system , 1987, Trends in Neurosciences.

[44]  P. Solomon,et al.  Latent inhibition and stimulus generalization of the classically conditioned nictitating membrane response in rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) following dorsal hippocampal ablation. , 1975, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[45]  R. C. Honey,et al.  Selective hippocampal lesions abolish the contextual specificity of latent inhibition and conditioning. , 1993, Behavioral neuroscience.

[46]  E T Rolls,et al.  Computational constraints suggest the need for two distinct input systems to the hippocampal CA3 network , 1992, Hippocampus.

[47]  R. C. Honey,et al.  Contextual effects in conditioning, latent inhibition, and habituation: Associative and retrieval functions of contextual cues , 1989 .

[48]  M. Gluck,et al.  Representation and Association in Memory: A Neurocomputational View of Hippocampal Function , 1995 .

[49]  G. Lynch,et al.  Variations in Synaptic Plasticity and Types of Memory in Corticohippocampal Networks , 1992, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[50]  N. Schmajuk,et al.  Stimulus configuration, classical conditioning, and hippocampal function. , 1992, Psychological review.

[51]  D. Woodruff-Pak,et al.  Eyeblink classical conditioning in H.M.: delay and trace paradigms. , 1993, Behavioral neuroscience.

[52]  G Lynch,et al.  Distinct memory circuits composing the hippocampal region , 1996, Hippocampus.

[53]  R. Sutherland,et al.  Configural association theory and the hippocampal formation: An appraisal and reconfiguration , 1995, Hippocampus.

[54]  M. Mishkin A memory system in the monkey. , 1982, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[55]  P. Solomon,et al.  Altered activity in the hippocampus is more detrimental to classical conditioning than removing the structure. , 1983, Science.

[56]  R. Sutherland,et al.  The hippocampal formation is necessary for rats to learn and remember configural discriminations , 1989, Behavioural Brain Research.

[57]  Howard Eichenbaum,et al.  Hippocampus and olfactory discrimination learning: effects of entorhinal cortex lesions on olfactory learning and memory in a successive-cue, go-no-go task. , 1991 .

[58]  W B Levy,et al.  A sequence predicting CA3 is a flexible associator that learns and uses context to solve hippocampal‐like tasks , 1996, Hippocampus.

[59]  S. D. Berry,et al.  Medial septal lesions retard classical conditioning of the nicitating membrane response in rabbits. , 1979, Science.

[60]  J. Murre TraceLink: A model of amnesia and consolidation of memory , 1996, Hippocampus.